Skip to main content

Changed health behavior improves subjective well-being and vice versa in a follow-up of 9 years

Abstract

Background

Previous research on health behavior and subjective well-being has mainly focused on interindividual differences or explored certain domains of health behavior. Good health behavior and subjective well-being at baseline can predict each other after a follow-up. In the present cohort study, we explored the outcomes of change for an individual i.e., how changed health behavior is reflected in subsequent subjective well-being and vice versa.

Methods

Data (n = 10,855) originates from a population-based Health and Social Support (HeSSup) study on working-age Finns in 2003 and 2012. A composite measure of health behavior included physical activity, dietary habits, alcohol consumption, and smoking status (range 0–4, worst–best) and a composite measure of subjective well-being (with reversed scoring) included three life assessments, i.e., interest, happiness, and ease in life, and perceived loneliness (range 4–20, best–worst). Different multiple linear regression models were used to study how changes in health behavior predict subjective well-being and the opposite, how changes in subjective well-being predict health behavior.

Results

A positive change in health behavior from 2003 to 2012 predicted better subjective well-being (i.e., on average 0.31 points lower subjective well-being sum score), whereas a negative change predicted poorer subjective well-being (i.e., 0.37 points higher subjective well-being sum score) (both: p < 0.001) compared to those study subjects who had no change in health behavior. Similarly, when a positive and negative change in subjective well-being was studied, these figures were 0.071 points better and 0.072 points worse (both: p < 0.001) health behavior sum score, respectively. When the magnitude of the effect of change was compared to the range of scale of the outcome the effect of health behavior change appeared stronger than that of subjective well-being.

Conclusion

Changes in health behavior and subjective well-being have long-term effects on the level of the other, the effect of the first being slightly stronger than vice versa. These mutual long-term benefits can be used as a motivator in health promotion on individual and societal levels.

Introduction

In general, subjective well-being (SWB) comprise of both a cognitive component, i.e. life satisfaction, and an affective component (i.e. both positive and negative affect) [1]. Thus, SWB measures can vary by their components or items. The cross-sectional association between domains of health behavior and different measures of SWB is well-established [2,3,4,5]. However, the bidirectional nature of this association has been addressed to require further research [6].

Changes in health behavior with respect to subsequent SWB and vice versa have been studied, but only in unidirectional settings. Increases in physical activity have co-occurred with changes in life satisfaction on a day-to-day basis [7] and in three major Socio-Economic Panels in Germany, Britain and Australia [8]. In the general adult population, an increase in the consumption of fruit and vegetable resulted in higher life satisfaction in a 5-year follow-up in UK [9] and in a 2-year follow-up in Australia [10].

On the other hand, changes in SWB have also been studied with respect to health behavior. In a 5-year follow-up of cardiovascular patients, increases in positive affect co-occurred with physical activity improvements, but not with changes in smoking status. However, baseline positive affect did not predict better health behavior, when baseline health behavior was controlled [11]. In a 4-week web-based follow-up, positive changes in life satisfaction and mood were both associated with decreases in dependence to smoking and higher rates of smoking cessation, whereas positive changes in life satisfaction with reduction in smoking, and positive changes in mood with decrease in feeling prisoner to cigarettes as well as smaller odds in smoking relapse [12].

Even if previous research is sparse on the role of change in the unidirectional setting between heath behavior and measures of SWB, there is a complete lack of studies on the role of their changes measured in bidirectional settings with the same data and with the same measures. Thus, the aim of the present study was to explore how change in a composite measure of health behavior predicts a subsequent composite measure of SWB and vice versa in a population-based sample of 10,000 working-age Finns.

Methods

Data (n = 10,855) originated from the second (in 2003) and third (in 2012) waves of a population-based Health and Social Support (HeSSup) study on working-age Finns. Unlike the first wave (in 1998), the questionnaires in these two waves included identical items in an identical or almost identical order for both SWB and health behavior. For details on response rates and the inclusion of participants see Fig. 1. Details of attrition of participants can be found elsewhere [13].

Fig. 1
figure 1

Inclusion and exclusion criteria for the study population. From Health and Social Support (HeSSup) prospective population-based follow-up study. an = 9986–10,786, due to missing responses on covariates. bn = 9986–10,855, due to missing responses on covariates. HBSS2003 = Health behavior sum score i.e., no. of protective health behaviors in 2003 (baseline). HBSS2012 = Health behavior sum score i.e., no. of protective health behaviors in 2012 (follow-up). SWB2003 = Subjective well-being in 2003 (baseline). SWB2012 = Subjective well-being in 2012 (follow-up)

Measures

A continuous health behavior sum score (HBSS, range 0–4) is a count of the number of beneficial health behaviors including physical activity, dietary habits, alcohol consumption, and smoking status. Physical activity performed in leisure time or in commuting was first converted into a Metabolic Equivalent Task (MET). A MET score ≥ 2 corresponding to 30 min walking per day was the cut-off value for beneficial behavior and provided one point in the HBSS. Dietary habits were assessed by a non-validated index (range 0–100), but formed in compliance with the Nordic nutritional recommendations [14]. Each choice, if being in line with the recommendations, provided one point in the dietary index according to the following cut-off points: dark bread (≥ 2/day); fat free milk (≥ 1/day); pastries/ sweets, sausages, red meat or chicken/ turkey (each ≤ 1–2/week); fish (≥ 1–2/week); fresh fruits and berries (≥ 2/day); vegetables (≥ 2/day); alcohol use (< 10 g women, 20 g men/day). Then, the sum score ranging from 0 to 10 was multiplied by 10 to give a percentage of compliance to recommendations [15]. Lastly, a cut-off value above median in this dietary index (≥ 60) provided one point for HBSS. Alcohol consumption was dichotomized according to Finnish guidelines [16] where risky consumption for women is ≥ 140 g/week and for men ≥ 280 g/week. Then, the values lower than the relevant cut-off provided one point for HBSS. Smoking status was dichotomized into current smokers vs. non-smokers in combination with former-smokers. The latter choice provided one point in HBSS.

Categorical change in health behavior (i.e., positive, neutral, and negative change) was measured by the change in the health behavior sum score (HBSSchange) during follow-up (i.e. the difference between HBSS2012 and HBSS2003). For evaluating change, individuals reporting zero or one beneficial health behaviors were combined into one group due to the small number of participants. Participants were categorized into three groups according to the direction of change in the HBSS during follow-up: positive, neutral, and negative change.

SWB was measured with the continuous four-item life satisfaction scale (range 4–20) [17, 18] where a lower score indicates better subjective well-being. It has three life assessments (i.e., interest, happiness, and ease in life) representing the cognitive component of SWB [1]. The fourth item is perceived loneliness, which is not typical for a life satisfaction scale, but reflects social well-being. The wordings of the life assessments were “Do you feel that your life at the moment is …”, and for the perceived loneliness “Do you feel that at the moment you are …? The responses were scored as follows: very interesting/ happy/ easy/ not at all lonely = 1; fairly interesting/ happy/ easy = 2; cannot say = 3; fairly boring/ unhappy/ hard/ lonely = 4; very boring/ unhappy/ hard/ lonely = 5 [18, 19]. Previously, the life satisfaction scale has often had three categories: satisfied (score = 4–6); intermediate group (score = 7–11, within ± 1 SD from the mean); dissatisfied (score = 12–20). In the present study, the intermediate group was divided at the mean to create four groups of SWB: high (score = 4–6), high intermediate (score = 7–8), low intermediate (score = 9–11), and low (score = 12–20).

The change in subjective well-being (SWBchange) during follow-up was assessed based on the direction of change between SWB groups with three categories (as in HBSSchange): positive, neutral, or negative change (each: yes/no). Thus, the SWBchange indicated the direction of change from one SWB group to another during the follow-up according to the difference in the group levels of SWB2012 and SWB2003, not in their continuous scores.

Factors potentially affecting both SWB and health behavior were included as covariates according to groupings based on the initial random sampling (age) and previous publications of the data. Participants represented four age groups in 2003: 25–29 years (group 1), 35–39 years (group 2), 45–49 years (group 3), and 55–59 years (group 4). Education was also categorized into four groups: (1) no professional education; (2) vocational course/school/apprenticeship contract; (3) college; (4) university degree/university of applied sciences. Health status was categorized according to count of the self-reported diseases into three groups: 0, 1, ≥ 2 diseases. Disease count is commonly used due to its simplicity and the ease of data ascertainment. Even if it does not consider the severity of disease, it is linked with mortality and various health adversities [20]. The complete list of 35 diseases can be found in the Additional file 1. Negative life events before the follow-up survey could affect both SWB and health behavior. From a list of 21 life events in 2007–2012, participants reported burdensome and extremely burdensome life events [21] which were then transformed into a trichotomized covariate: 0, 1, ≥ 2 major negative life events. Details of the life events and the item in the survey can be found in the Additional file 1.

Statistical analyses

Linear regression models were used to analyze:

  1. (1)

    The effect of the categorical HBSSchange on the association between baseline HBSS (continuous HBSS2003) and follow-up SWB (continuous SWBscore2012)

  2. (2)

    The effect of categorical SWBchange on the association between baseline SWB (continuous SWBscore2003) and follow-up HBSS (continuous HBSS2012).

As already described, both HBSSchange and SWBchange were measured by the difference between their baseline and end of follow-up grouping divided into three categories (i.e. positive, neutral, and negative change). The first digit of the adjustment models identified the direction (1: from HBSS2003 to SWB2012; 2: from SWB2003 to HBSS2012) and the second digit was for the adjustment factors. Thus, the Model 1.1. from HBSS2003 to SWB2012 was adjusted for age, gender, education, and disease count (Table 3). The additional adjustments were for either HBSSchange (Model 1.2), or SWB2003 (Model 1.3) or both (Final model 1.4.). The Model 1.4 was then further adjusted with negative life events to create Model 1.5 (For clarification see Table 3). To study the effect of change without baseline level, HBSSchange was made the predictor instead of baseline HBSS2003 to create Model 1.6, which is otherwise parallel to Model 1.1. The Model 1.7 was created by adding also SWB2003 in Model 1.6. (Table 3).

In the opposite direction (from SWB2003 to HBSS2012) (Table 5), the Model 2.1 was adjusted for age, gender, education, and disease count as in Model 1.1., but it had also the SWB2003*education interaction term (Table 5). The additional adjustments were for either SWBchange (Model 2.2) or HBSS2003 (Model 2.3) or both (Final model 2.4). The Model 2.4 was then further adjusted with negative life events to create Model 2.5. To study the effect of change without baseline level, SWBchange was made the predictor instead of baseline SWB2003 and the interaction term SWB2003*education was converted to SWBchange*education to create Model 2.6 from the Model 2.1. The model 2.7 was created by adding also HBSS2003 in the Model 2.6. (Table 5). Data were analyzed with SAS software (version 9.4; SAS Institute Inc. Cary, NC, USA 2016).

To be able to compare the effects of the SWBchange and HBSSchange on their outcomes i.e. HBSS2012 (range 0–4 with 4 steps) and SWB2012 (range 4–20 with 16 steps), the HBSS2012 should be rescaled. Thus, the estimate for SWBchange is multiplied by 4 (4 × 4 = 16) to be comparable for the estimate of HBSSchange.

Results

During the 9-year follow-up the average HBSS for the entire study population improved from 2.87 to 2.99. A positive HBSSchange was observed in 30.5% (n = 3100), no change in 48.3% (n = 4913), and deterioration in 21.2% (n = 2160). For details, see Table 1. Average SWB improved from score 8.53 to 8.37 (lower scores indicating better SWB). Its greatest improvements were observed in the oldest age group and in individuals experiencing no major negative life events. A positive SWBchange was observed in 30.2% of the study population (n = 3546), neutral change in 44.0% (n = 5172), and negative change in 25.8% (n = 3034). For details, see Table 2. Details of the distribution of individual health behaviors at baseline can be found elsewhere [13].

Table 1 Health behavior sum score at baseline (HBSS2003) and after follow-up (HBSS2012) as well as its changes (HBSSchange) by subgroup, as percentages (numbers), unless otherwise stated. Results of the Finnish population-based Health and Social Support Study
Table 2 Subjective well-being at baseline (SWBscore2003) and after follow-up (SWBscore2012) as well as its changes (SWBchange) by subgroup, as percentages (numbers), unless otherwise stated. Results of the Finnish population-based Health and Social Support Study

HBSSchange predicting SWB2012

HBSS2003 predicted the SWB2012 in each model (Table 3), the β being – 0.35 (p < 0.001, Akaike information criterion (AIC) = 48,300) in the final model 1.5 (adjusted for age, gender, education, disease count, HBSSchange, SWB2003 and negative life events). A positive HBSSchange predicted 0.31 (p < 0.001) points lower SWBscore2012 (i.e., better SWB) and a negative change predicted 0.37 (p < 0.001) points higher SWBscore2012 after follow-up compared to those with no change in the HBSS2003 during follow-up (Table 4). In addition, every one point increase in the HBSS2003 (β = 0.35; p < 0.001) resulted in a 0.35 points lower SWBscore2012 (indicating better SWB) and every higher point in the SWBscore2003 (β = 0.42; p < 0.001) resulted in a 0.42 points higher SWBscore2012. With the reference group being at least two major negative life events in 2007–2012, having one or none major negative life event resulted separetely analyzed in overall 0.75 (p < 0.001) and 1.08 (p < 0.001) points lower SWBscore2012 (indicating better SWB), respectively. Further, lower disease count in 2003 (p < 0.001) resulted in better SWB2012 compared to those with at least two diseases. Age as a covariate (p < 0.001) resulted in a U-shaped pattern in SWB2012 (age group 2 having worst SWB2012). In addition, education was a significant covariate (p = 0.038) but not gender. Using HBSSchange as a predictor instead of HBSS2003 in Model 1.1. (Model 1.6) or adding baseline SWBscore2003 in it (Model 1.7), did not improve the AIC or statistical significance of covariates. (Table 3).

Table 3 Linear regression models in which baseline health behavior (HBSS2003) or its change (HBSSchange) predicts follow-up subjective well-being (SWBscore2012)
Table 4 Estimates of the linear regression model 1.5 in which health behavior (HBSS2003) predicts subjective well-being (SWBscore2012) Results of Finnish population-based Health and Social Support study

SWBchange predicting HBSS

In the final model 2.4 (adjusted for age, gender, education, disease count, HBSSchange and HBSS2003) β was –0.025 (p < 0.001, AIC = 23,000). A positive SWBchange resulted in 0.071 (p < 0.001) points higher HBSS2012 and a negative change 0.072 points lower HBSS2012 compared to the ones who experienced a neutral SWBchange during follow-up. Every point towards better SWB2003 (decrease in score) improved HBSS2012 by 0.025 points, and every additional point in HBSS2003 improved the HBSS2012 by 0.48 points. Better HBSS2012 was observed in women (p < 0.001) (vs. men) and those with no diseases (p < 0.03) (vs. ≥ 2 diseases). Further, having a university level education had a statistically significant effect on the interaction SWB2003*education (p = 0.005). Age appeared to have a U-shaped effect on subsequent health behavior, but it lost its significance when covariates were added. Education by itself was not a significant covariate. For details of different models, see Table 5, and for final model 2.4, see Table 6. The model did not improve when adjusting for negative life events (p = 0.52) or when SWBscore2003 was replaced by SWBchange and the interaction SWBchange*education included in the Model 2.6 (Table 5).

Table 5 Linear regression models in which baseline subjective well-being (SWBscore2003) or its change (SWBchange) predicts follow-up health behavior (HBSS2012)
Table 6 Linear regression model 2.4 in which subjective well-being (SWBscore2003) predicts health behavior (HBSS2012)

Comparison of the directions of effect in the final models

The range of the score depicting SWB (4–20 i.e., 16 different points) is four times the magnitude of range of HBSS (0–4). When comparing the magnitudes of effect, the negative HBSSchange had a greater effect (0.37 points) on SWB2012 than a negative SWBchange on HBSS2012 (4*0.072 points = 0.288 points). A positive HBSSchange resulted in slightly greater effect (0.31points) compared to a positive SWBchange (4*0.071 points = 0.284 points). Thus, it appears that changes in HBSS have stronger effect on SWB than vice versa. The maximum effect of SWB2003 (range 4–20) on HBSS2012 (range 0–4) could be 16*0.02548 (i.e., -0.25 in Table 6) ≈ 0.41 and the maximum effect of HBSS2003 on SWB2012 was 4*0.3515 (i.e., -0.35 in Table 4) ≈ 1.4.

Discussion

In the present study on 10,000 working-age Finns, a change in health behavior appears to have a slightly stronger effect on subsequent SWB than vice versa when adjusting for age, gender, education, diseases, baseline level of outcome, and negative life events prior to the follow-up. In addition, it was observed that on average both health behavior and SWB improve during the 9-year follow-up.

In both directions of effect, change in the predictor had a statistically significant effect on the outcome. When comparing the effect of change to the effect of baseline level, the baseline level of health behavior and SWB seem to be stronger predictors in the models than their changes. The effect of health behavior change (assessing only the direction of change) in the 9-year follow-up on subsequent SWB was of approximately equal magnitude to the effect of one point difference in the baseline health behavior and therefore about a fourth of the maximum possible effect of baseline health behavior. The effect of SWB change (assessing only the direction of change) in the 9-year follow-up on subsequent health behavior was about three times greater than the effect of one point better baseline SWB but about a sixth of the maximum difference in the baseline SWB. Furthermore, adjusting for health behavior change strengthened the effect of baseline health behavior and adjusting for SWB change strengthens the effect of baseline SWB.

Our results are in line with earlier research where increase in fruit and vegetable consumption has resulted in better life satisfaction even though the magnitude of the average effects was small. In 50,000 individuals in the UK, an increase of one portion of fruits and vegetables in a follow-up of 5-years resulted in a 0.133 point increase in well-being (p < 0.001, range of scale 0–36) [9]. In a follow-up of 2 years, an increased consumption of 8 portions of fruits and vegetables increased life satisfaction by 0.24 points (on a scale of 0–10), which, however, was estimated to be of similar magnitude to the effect of moving from unemployment to employment [10]. In earlier studies, bidirectional relationship of health or health behavior and SWB has been analyzed with the same method [22] and by cross-lagged panel models previously [23,24,25]. However, none of these studies explore the effect of intraindividual change.

The opposite directions of effects have distinct characteristics. When health behavior predicts SWB, an action-based characteristic is predicting a subjective evaluation and perception of life. Thus, it is reasonable that negative life events can have a significant effect on SWB, which was true in the present study. In contrast, negative life events did not affect subsequent health behavior. Age showed a significant effect on subsequent life satisfaction when adjusted for sociodemographic factors, health behavior and major diseases, which is in line with earlier research [1, 26]. However, age was not a significant covariate in our studies when life satisfaction predicted health behavior which also has been less explored in research. For example, the consumption of fruit and vegetables has shown a reversed U-shaped curve with the highest consumption around the age of 60 years [9]. In our study, the observed U-shaped pattern in health behavior by age could be caused by SWB that affects health behavior and also follows a U-shaped curve by age. This could imply that age in itself does not determine health behavior. Gender was a significant covariate when SWB predicted health behavior but not in the opposite direction. Personality, genetics or family upbringing, could affect both health behavior [27,28,29,30] and SWB [31]. However, these effects should stay constant during follow-up [as in 9].

Change in health behavior appears to have a significant effect on subsequent SWB and vice versa. In addition to interindividual differences, changes within an individual had an impact on the relationship between health behavior and SWB. An explanation for improvement in health behavior along with SWB might go through enhanced self-efficacy, which has been reported as a result of increased positive affect in a behavior change intervention [32]. Improved health behavior, on the other hand, can improve health and therefore also potentially SWB. Thus, change in both or in either health behavior or SWB is beneficial, and can be used to motivate personal health promotion. A change in health behavior can lead to both reduced costs of healthcare and – through improved SWB – also to better productivity, which is an interest in political decision making [31]. As in previous studies on this data [25] the results could be generalized to the working-age population in Finland and presumably in other Western countries.

Strengths and weaknesses

The present study is to our knowledge the first to explore the effect of individual change in the bidirectional setting of multiple health behaviors and a measure of SWB. Thus, it provides more understanding in the complex relationship between health behavior and SWB. A consistent survey procedure and large population-based sample yield solid results. There is adequate representation of covariate groups and individuals showing also negative changes in health behavior and SWB, who usually participate less in health-related surveys. In addition, a follow-up of 9 years is a long enough time perspective in an adult’s life. Thus, a statistically significant effect of that size after such a follow-up is considerable.

The study has also limitations. Attrition might have caused some overrepresentation of positive changes as well as better baseline levels of HBSS and SWB. The magnitude of change was not quantified. Only the direction of change was used in the analysis. Therefore, firm conclusions of the effect of a specific change cannot be drawn. The use of only two follow-up points might be subject to bias of momentary external factors. However, the large data could eradicate most of the effects of individual factors in people’s lives. The study is observational in nature. Thus, definite conclusions about causality cannot be made. The measures are obtained by self-report which especially for health behavior could be subject to reporting-bias [33]. Even if self-reported diseases lack the severity assessments, disease count is the most common instrument to measure the level of multimorbidity being also linked with various health outcomes according to a recent systematic review [20].

Further research

Changes can be explored in predictor, outcome or in both. Using the same data and different approaches allowing bidirectional analyses provides more knowledge on this area. Change could be quantified in more detail than in the present study. Secondly, different indicators for SWB should be studied when assessing the role of change in its association with health behavior. Such studies could contribute to acknowledge the potential of SWB in health behavior interventions. Thirdly, future research is needed to identify which characteristics or factors affect the change to identify those who could benefit from SWB aspect in health behavior interventions.

Conclusion

A change in health behavior predicts subsequent SWB and vice versa in a follow-up of 9 years. The effect of health behavior change on SWBs is slightly stronger than vice versa.

Availability of data and materials

The dataset analyzed is not publicly available due to the study data containing variables of personal and sensitive nature and hence, due to the present legislation of Finland and the General Data Protection Regulation (GDPR) of the European Union, cannot be made openly accessible inside or outside Finland. In special cases data is available from the authors on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

HBSS:

Health behavior sum score

HBSSchange:

Change in health behavior sum score during follow-up

SWB:

Subjective well-being

SWBscore:

Subjective well-being score

SWBchange:

Change in subjective well-being subgroup during follow-up

References

  1. Diener E, Oishi S, Tay L. Advances in subjective well-being research. Nat Hum Behav. 2018;2:253–60. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0307-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Kushlev K, Drummond DM, Diener E. Subjective well-being and health behaviors in 2.5 million Americans. Appl Psychol Heal Well-Being. 2019;12:166–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Grant N, Wardle J, Steptoe A. The relationship between life satisfaction and health behavior: a cross-cultural analysis of young adults. Int J Behav Med. 2009;16:259–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Strine TW, Chapman DP, Balluz LS, Moriarty DG, Mokdad AH. The associations between life satisfaction and health-related quality of life, chronic illness, and health behaviors among U.S. community-dwelling adults. J Community Health. 2008;33:40–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Liu B, Floud S, Pirie K, Green J, Peto R, Beral V. Does happiness itself directly affect mortality? The prospective UK Million Women Study. Lancet. 2016;387:874–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(15)01087-9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Diener E, Pressman SD, Hunter J, Delgadillo-Chase D. If, why, and when subjective well-being influences health, and future needed research. Appl Psychol Heal Well-Being. 2017;9:133–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Conroy DE, Ram N, Pincus AL, Coffman DL, Lorek AE, Rebar AL, et al. Daily physical activity and alcohol use across the adult lifespan. Heal Psychol. 2014. (Advance online publication).

  8. Headey B, Muffels R, Wagner GG. Choices which change life satisfaction: similar results for Australia, Britain and Germany. Soc Indic Res. 2012;112:725–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Ocean N, Howley P, Ensor J. Lettuce be happy: a longitudinal UK study on the relationship between fruit and vegetable consumption and well-being. Soc Sci Med. 2019;222:335–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Mujcic R, Oswald AJ. Evolution of well-being and happiness after increases in consumption of fruit and vegetables. Public Health. 2016;106:1504–10.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Sin NL, Moskowitz JT, Whooley MA. Positive affect and health behaviors across 5 years in patients with coronary heart disease: the heart and soul study. Psychosom Med. 2015;77:1058–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Haller CS. Trajectories of smoking behavior as a function of mood and satisfaction with life: what matters most? J Affect Disord. 2016;190:407–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Stenlund S, Koivumaa-Honkanen H, Sillanmäki L, Lagström H, Rautava P, Suominen S. Health behavior of working-aged Finns predicts self-reported life satisfaction in a population-based 9-years follow-up. BMC Public Health. 2021;21(1):1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Becker W, Lyhne N, Pedersen AN, Aro A, Fogelholm M, Phórsdottir I, et al. Nordic nutrition recommendations 2004—Integrating nutrition and physical activity. Scand J Nutr. 2004;48:178–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Lagström H, Halonen JI, Kawachi I, Stenholm S, Pentti J, Suominen S, et al. Neighborhood socioeconomic status and adherence to dietary recommendations among Finnish adults: a retrospective follow-up study. Heal Place. 2019;55:43–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Duodecim. Alkoholiongelmaisen hoito. Käypä hoito-suositus. Suomalainen Lääkäriseura Duodecim; 2018. https://www.kaypahoito.fi/hoi50028.

  17. Allardt E. Dimensions of welfare in a comparative. Source Acta Sociol. 1976;19(3):227–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Koivumaa-Honkanen H, Honkanen R, Viinamäki H, Heikkilä K, Kaprio J, Koskenvuo M. Self-reported life satisfaction and 20-year mortality in healthy finnish adults. Am J Epidemiol. 2000;152(10):983–91.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Koivumaa-Honkanen H, Kaprio J, Korhonen T, Honkanen RJ, Heikkilä K, Koskenvuo M. Self-reported life satisfaction and alcohol use: a 15-year follow-up of healthy adult twins. Alcohol Alcohol. 2012;47:160–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Lee ES, Koh HL, Ho EQY, Teo SH, Wong FY, Ryan BL, et al. Systematic review on the instruments used for measuring the association of the level of multimorbidity and clinically important outcomes. BMJ Open. 2021;11:1–21.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Vahtera J, Kivimäki M, Hublin C, Korkeila K, Suominen S, Paunio T, et al. Liability to anxiety and severe life events as predictors of new-onset sleep disturbances. Sleep. 2007;30:1537–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Stenlund S, Koivumaa-Honkanen H, Sillanmäki L, Lagström H, Rautava P, Suominen S. Subjective well-being predicts health behavior in a population-based 9-years follow-up of working-aged Finns. Prev Med Rep. 2021;24:101635.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Lappan S, Thorne CB, Long D, Hendricks PS. Longitudinal and reciprocal relationships between psychological well-being and smoking. Nicotine Tob Res. 2018;22:18–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Wiese CW, Chen ZJ, Tay L, Friedman EM, Rector JL. The role of affect on physical health over time: a cross-lagged panel analysis over 20 years. Appl Psychol Heal Well-Being. 2019;11:202–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Stenlund S, Junttila N, Koivumaa-Honkanen H, Sillanmaki L, Stenlund D, Suominen S, et al. Longitudinal stability and interrelations between health behavior and subjective well- being in a follow-up of nine years. PLoS ONE. 2021;16(10):e0259280.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. López Ulloa BF, Møller V, Sousa-Poza A. How does subjective well-being evolve with age? A literature review. J Popul Ageing. 2013;6:227–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Kestilä L, Martelin T, Rahkonen O, Joutsenniemi K, Pirkola S, Poikolainen K, et al. Childhood and current determinants of heavy drinking in early adulthood. Alcohol Alcohol. 2008;43:460–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Kestilä L, Koskinen S, Martelin T, Rahkonen O, Pensola T, Pirkola S, et al. Influence of parental education, childhood adversities, and current living conditions on daily smoking in early adulthood. Eur J Public Health. 2006;16:617–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Proyer RT, Gander F, Wellenzohn S, Ruch W. What good are character strengths beyond subjective well-being? The contribution of the good character on self-reported health-oriented behavior, physical fitness, and the subjective health status. J Posit Psychol. 2013;8:222–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Booth-Kewley S, Vickers RR. Associations between major domains of personality and health behavior. J Pers. 1994;62:281–98.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Diener E. The Remarkable Changes in the Science of Subjective Well-Being. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2013;8:663–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Charlson ME, Wells MT, Peterson JC, Boutin-Foster C, Ogedegbe GO, Mancuso CA, et al. Mediators and moderators of behavior change in patients with chronic cardiopulmonary disease: the impact of positive affect and self-affirmation. Transl Behav Med. 2014;4:7–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Wasserman RM, Hilliard ME. Health behavior change research methodology. In: Hilliard ME, Riekert KA, Ockene JK, Pbert L, editors. The handbook of health behavior change. 5th ed. Berlin: Springer; 2018.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable

Funding

Grants received from TYKS foundation (SSt), the Signe and Ane Gyllenberg foundation (SSt; HKH), the Waldemar von Frenckel foundation (SSt), the Päivikki and Sakari Sohlberg foundation (HKH), and the Yrjö Jahnsson foundation (SSt). The funding sources did not participate in the design or conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis or interpretation of the data; or preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

SSt presented the original idea for the study, planned statistical analysis, evaluated and interpreted the results of the analysis, wrote the first draft of the manuscript, and contributed to the revision at all stages of the writing process. SSu developed the original research question, planned the statistical analysis, interpreted the results of the analysis, and commented all the revisions of the manuscript. HK-H developed the original research question, contributed to the planning of the statistical analysis, contributed to the interpretation of the results of the analysis, and commented all the revisions of the manuscript. LS participated in the planning of statistical analyses, carried out the statistical analysis, and contributed to the revision of the manuscript. HL developed the dietary index, took part in the planning of the research method, and contributed to the revision of the manuscript. PR contributed to the development of the original research question and contributed to the revision of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Säde Stenlund.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The concurrent joint Ethical Committee of the University of Turku and the Turku University Central Hospital approved the Health and Social Support study. The study was carried out according to the Declaration of Helsinki. Participants signed a written consent agreeing to a prospective follow-up. The use of data from the Health and Social Support study in our research did not require additional ethics approval but was granted by the study group led by the Principal Investigator, Professor Markku Koskenvuo. All study members had to sign a written commitment to data protection and all shared and analyzed data was before delivery pseudonymized.

Consent for publish

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

List of diseases and life events.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Stenlund, S., Koivumaa-Honkanen, H., Sillanmäki, L. et al. Changed health behavior improves subjective well-being and vice versa in a follow-up of 9 years. Health Qual Life Outcomes 20, 66 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12955-022-01972-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12955-022-01972-4

Keywords