Skip to main content

What were they thinking when providing preference measurements for generic health states? The evidence for HUI3

Background

Multi-attribute generic preference-based measures of health-related quality of life are used as comprehensive outcome measures. Typically preferences for health states defined by these systems are elicited from a representative sample of the general population. An important element in that elicitation process is the information that respondents were instructed to consider in providing their responses.

Methods

A random sample of community-dwelling respondents in Canada was surveyed in face-to-face interviews. Respondents provided preference scores for selected Health Utilities Index Mark 3 (HUI3) health states. Respondents also answered questions about the most important attributes and the importance of various impacts of the health states in providing their preference scores.

Results

Fifty per cent of respondents reported that they focussed on two, and 21% on three, attributes of the eight HUI3 attributes. Each of the eight attributes was identified as important; pain (49%), vision (37%), cognition (34%), emotion (28%), and ambulation (28%) were the most important. The null hypothesis that all of the attributes were equally important was rejected (pā€‰<ā€‰0.001). With respect to the impacts, 89% of respondents indicated that the ability to take care of oneself was quite or very important; similarly 76% reported the same for impact on family life, 69% for impact on the happiness of others, 61% for the impact on their ability to work, and 42% for the impact on their leisure activities. The null hypothesis that all of the impacts were equally important was rejected (pā€‰<ā€‰0.001).

Conclusions

In providing preference scores for HUI3 health states, respondents thoughtfully examined the implications of the health states for their ability to live, work, socialize, and function.

Introduction

Generic multi-attribute preference-based measures of health-related quality of life (HRQL) are widely used as comprehensive outcome measures in clinical studies, including economic evaluations and health technology assessments of healthcare, and in population health surveys. By convention, scores of these measures are on a scale in which deadā€‰=ā€‰0.00 and perfect healthā€‰=ā€‰1.00; therefore these measures can be used to integrate mortality and morbidity as is done in calculating summary measures of health such as quality-adjusted life years (QALYs). Prominent examples of these measures include the EQ-5D-3Ā L [21], the Health Utilities Index Mark 2 (HUI2) [28], the Health Utilities Index Mark 3 (HUI3) [12], the Quality of Well Being Scale (QWB) [16], and the Short-Form 6D (SF-6D) [1].

Each of these multi-attribute systems has a health-status classification system that describes a large number of health states and a multi-attribute utility function that provides a preference-based score for each of those health states. There is substantial evidence on the validity of the health-status classification systems and the multi-attribute utility function scoring systems for these measures [6].

However, relatively little information is reported in the literature on what respondents who provided the preference scores used to estimate these multi-attribute utility functions were instructed to consider. Key to the appropriate interpretation and use of any multi-attribute preference-based scores is an understanding of how the scores were elicited and what factors respondents considered in providing their valuations. This paper provides such information for HUI3.

The importance of such information is highlighted in guidelines for reporting standards for utility assessment [24] and multi-attribute utility-based instruments [29]. Stalmeier and colleagues specify 25 categories (many with sub-categories) of information that should be reported, as applicable. Relevant with respect to the theme of this paper is C12 (p 204): ā€œWhat was the subject instructed to assume, if anything, regarding costs to him or her or family about the possible outcomes?ā€ Xie and colleagues in discussing reporting guidelines on scoring functions for multi-attribute preference-based measures list 21 items classified in seven sections. Of relevance here is Item 11 (p 872): ā€œMode of Data Collection is Stated.ā€ More generally, McDowell and Newell [18] note that ā€œwe know relatively little about the process of making subjective health judgementsā€.

An example of a report on the conduct of a preference elicitation survey that reports on the experiences of respondents is Devlin et al. [2]. Devlin and colleagues summarize the written comments provided by respondents to an EQ-5D-3Ā L postal survey in New Zealand and conclude that ā€œthe valuation exercise imposes a substantial cognitive burden on respondents and many do not understand itā€ (p 1265).

An example of an important reason for knowing what factors respondents were instructed to consider is the controversy about the extent to which the productivity consequences of health states are reflected in preference scores. The original Panel on Cost Effectiveness in Health and Medicine assumed that the effects of morbidity on productivity were incorporated in the preference scores and therefore the monetary (pecuniary) consequences need not be estimated separately ([13], pp. 181ā€“182). The Panel argued that including the monetary estimates would result in double counting. Several subsequent studies have examined the extent to which the effects of health on productivity in the labor market and household production are reflected in the preference scores, casting doubt on the original Panelā€™s position [17, 23, 27]. Sculpher and Oā€™Brien [22] report that respondents in the Measurement and Valuation of Health study (used to estimate the scoring UK function for the EQ-5D-3Ā L; see [3]) were given no explicit guidance on this issue. The Second Panel on Cost Effectiveness in Health and Medicine concluded that it is unlikely that productivity effects are indeed captured in the utility scores and instead recommends that these effects be captured in monetary terms [19]. This is an example of why what respondents were or were not instructed to consider is important information for interpreting the scores.

The aims of this paper include reporting on the factors respondents were instructed to consider, and did consider, in providing preference scores used to estimate a multiplicative multi-attribute utility function for the HUI3 system [4, 11]. Another aim is a recommendation that the current reporting guidelines be expanded to include a description of what factors respondents were instructed to consider and whether or not respondents were debriefed about what they were thinking while providing their preference scores. This paper is based on information derived from the debriefing of respondents in the HUI3 scoring project. The results of the debriefing have implications for the content and construct validity of the HUI3 system. The paper reports previously unreported results from a broader study. The relevant methods from the broader study are described briefly in the Background section, followed by a description of the methods, results, discussion, and conclusions for the specific topics upon which this paper focuses.

Background

HUI3 consists of two major components: a health-status classification system; and a preference-based scoring function for valuing the health states described by that system. The health-status classification system describes an individualā€™s health status at a point in time based on eight attributes (or dimensions or domains) of health. The attributes in conventional order are: vision (V), hearing (H), speech (S), ambulation (A), dexterity (D), emotion (E), cognition (C) and pain and discomfort (P). There are five or six levels per attribute, varying from normal to severe disability. For example, pain varies from ā€œfree of pain and discomfortā€ to ā€œsevere pain that prevents most activitiesā€. (The full HUI3 health-status classification system is shown in the Appendix.) An individualā€™s comprehensive health state is described as an eight-element vector, one level for each attribute; an example is provided in Fig.Ā 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Example of an HUI3 Health-State Description Card. Legend of symbols for attribute levels: no symbol ā€“ attribute at level 1 (no disability); circle ā€“ level 2; triangle ā€“ level 3; square ā€“ level 4; diamond (not shown) ā€“ level 5 if attribute has 6 levels; asterisk ā€“ lowest functional level, level 5 if attribute has 5 levels or level 6 if the attribute has 6 levels. Note. A health-state description consists of one level for each of the eight HUI3 attributes: vision, hearing, speech, ambulation, dexterity, emotion, cognition, and pain and discomfort. In vector notation, the health state above is written as V2,H1,S1,A1,D1,E2,C3,P5

The second major component is the scoring function that provides utility scores on the conventional dead to perfect health scale for all of the health states defined by the health-status classification system. The scoring function is based on directly measured preference scores obtained from a random sample of the general population survey respondents (community preferences). The procedures for eliciting the preference scores from the respondents are described briefly below. More comprehensive descriptions are found in Furlong et al. [11] and Feeny et al. [4].

Respondents were asked to score a series of HUI3 health states. Respondents were told that they would live in each of the various HUI3 health states described for the rest of their life, which was explicitly defined as the duration of each respondentā€™s self-nominated additional life expectancy in years. The interview script explicitly instructed respondents to focus only on the health-state descriptions and to keep all other factors constant: ā€œWhen imagining yourself in these health states please remember that where you live, your income, your friends, and family would be the same as now.ā€

The HUI3 preference measurement survey included two widely used preference elicitation instruments: a visual analog scale known as the Feeling Thermometer (FT) and the standard gamble implemented using the Chance Board (CB). (The FT is presented as Fig. 1 in [10], p 82; the CB as Fig. 2, p 83.Ā http://www.chepa.org/Files/Working%20Papers/WP%2090-9.pdf). Respondents were asked to rank order a set of HUI3 health states on the FT in which the top of the scale was labelled ā€œMost Desirableā€ and the bottom of the scale was labelled ā€œLeast Desirableā€. FigureĀ 1 is an example of how health-state descriptions were presented to respondents. In particular, note the use of symbols to assist respondents in identifying the attributes for which there was any disability and the extent of that disability. The number of health states which a respondent was asked to evaluate on the FT varied from 14 to 18 depending on the survey group to which the respondent was randomized. A sub-set of four to nine states (depending on the survey group to which the respondent was randomized) were subsequently measured using the CB. With the CB the respondent was offered a lottery, Choice A, consisting of perfect health state (all attributes at level 1, no disability) with probability p and the least preferable health state (either the all-worst health state (all attributes at their lowest level, highest degree of disability) or being dead, depending on the CB question) with probability 1- p versus Choice B, an intermediately ranked state, with probability pā€‰=ā€‰1.00 (i.e., for sure). Probability p in Choice A is varied systematically until the subject is indifferent between the Choice A and Choice B. The more desirable the health state in Choice B, the higher the indifference probability.

After completing the FT and CB tasks, respondents were asked nine demographic questions. Respondents were then asked 11 debriefing questions, each with multiple-choice or open-ended response options about their opinions regarding the importance of health-status attributes and impact factors in the preference measurement interview process. Assessments of debriefing questions having quantifiable responses are the focus of this paper. Note that the debriefing questions were asked after the questions that elicited scores for the HUI3 health states presented to that respondent.

The preference-elicitation survey instrument for the HUI3 scoring function project involved detailed word-by-word interview scripting of instructions to respondents, use of well-tested props, in-person interviewing, intensive training of professional interviewers and provision of a custom interviewer manual, pre-testing and practice interviews, and audio-recording of all interviews for quality assurance. The survey instructions focussed on health-state descriptions, the context for the preference measurement of health states (for instance, the instruction that ā€œyour income, your friends, and family would be the same as nowā€), and the preference measurement techniques that respondents were to use for the health states presented to them. The survey instrument was designed to minimize the consideration of extraneous factors and minimize intra- and inter-interviewer effects. All these factors are important design components of the preference elicitation survey that should be reported so that users of the resulting scoring function can make informed judgments about the methodologic rigour and quality of the foundational preference measurements.

Methods

This paper focuses on the respondent debriefing questions with quantifiable response options asking respondents about the importance of the health attributes and impact factors they were thinking about when responding to the preference measurement questions (Table 1). The preference measurement interview structure was predicated on the assumption that respondents have well developed but not readily articulable preferences for health states and, therefore they need an opportunity to think and reflect before providing their response. The interview process was structured to give the respondent ample opportunity and a structured set of tasks to help them contemplate, discover, and state their preferences. Thus respondents were asked to describe their own health by identifying the most appropriate level for each attribute of the HUI3 health-status classification system, and the rank ordering and rating using the FT was followed by further intense reflection by having to make choices on the CB.

Table 1 Respondent Interview Debriefing Questionsa

Preference measurement scores for the HUI3 health states were obtained in a survey of a random sample of the English-speaking general population (age 16Ā years and older) in the city of Hamilton, Ontario, Canada. One debriefing question asked if respondents focussed on one or two health-state characteristics (attributes). We investigate whether all attributes are considered important, and test the null hypothesis that all attributes are equally important to preference measurement at the group level. We also compare the importance ratings of the attributes to the prevalence of disability in those attributes using data on the prevalence of any disability among the respondents (nā€‰=ā€‰504) and in the general population (from the [25], nā€‰=ā€‰11,924).

Another question asked respondents about the importance of five potential impacts on activities of living in their scoring of the health states. We measured the importance of each of the five factors and test the null hypotheses that each of the five factors is important and that the five factors are of equal importance.

The minimum level of statistical significance is pā€‰<ā€‰0.05. The minimum size for difference in rates regarded as important is 5 %. Chi-square tests were used to test differences in rates (i.e., proportions).

Results

The demographic characteristics of the sample (nā€‰=ā€‰504 respondents) are presented in Table 2. Respondents varied in age from 16 to 85 with a mean of 43Ā years. Nearly 60% were female and most reported good or better health. The frequency distributions of the demographic characteristics for the sample of respondents are very similar to the census distributions reported by Statistics Canada for the general community-dwelling population [4, 11].

Table 2 Socio-Demographic Characteristics of Respondents (nā€‰=ā€‰504)

The professional interviewers reported that 74.4% of respondents were extremely cooperative; for 76.2% of respondents the degree of thought was a great deal or quite a bit; and for 75.8% the overall quality of the interviews was very good or good ([4], pp. 122ā€“123).

When asked if there were particular health attributes upon which the respondent focussed, there was considerable variation among respondents and attributes (Table 3). Each of the attributes was reported as being of particular importance in making preference measurement decisions by 5% or more of respondents. The reporting rates of particular importance varied substantially among attributes (pā€‰<ā€‰0.001) and there were important size (>ā€‰5%) differences in rates among attributes. For example the rate for the most frequently reported attribute of particular importance (pain at 49%) was approximately seven times the rate for the attribute least frequently reported (speech at 6.9%). Furthermore, most respondents reported multiple attributes of particular importance: 50% of respondents reported two attributes and 21% of respondents reported three attributes. Six per cent of respondents said that none of the attributes were more important than the rest.

Table 3 Attribute Importance and Rates of Disability by Attribute

Table 3 reports the frequency of any disability for each attribute as reported by respondents to the preference measurement survey (Column 2) and in a large general population health survey. Attribute importance rates of preference survey respondents (Column 1) differed significantly (pā€‰<ā€‰0.001) from both the disability rates of the preference survey respondents (Column 2) and disability rates of the general population health survey respondents (Column 3). While vision had the highest rate of disability, pain was rated as the most important attribute. Further, while 28% rated ambulation as particularly important, the rates of disability in ambulation were 12.3% among preference survey respondents and 3% in the general population. Pain, vision, and cognition were the three attributes most frequently reported to be particularly important and among the four attributes with the highest prevalence of disability in both the preference survey respondent and the general population samples.

Each of the five impact factors was considered at least somewhat important (pā€‰<ā€‰0.003) by 70% or more of respondents when making FT and CB preference measurement decisions (Table 4). Some impact factors were reported as being important more frequently than others (pā€‰<ā€‰0.001). The impact factor most frequently reported to be important was the ability to look after oneself (96%). The least frequently reported was leisure activities (71%).

Table 4 Importance of Impact Factors: Ranking from Most to Least Importance

Finally, when asked if their opinions had changed during the interview process, most respondents (83%) reported that their opinions or strength of their opinions and feelings had not changed at a result of the interview.

Discussion

The preference measurement interviews involved numerous challenging tasks. On the Feeling Thermometer, respondents are instructed to rank health states on a vertical visual analogue scale and ensure that the distance between states on the scale reflected the relative strength of their preference differences among states. On the Chance Board respondents were asked to compare their preference for health state in Choice B to the their preference for the probabilities of perfect health and, in general, the all-worst state in Choice A.

As noted above, the professional interviewers considered more than 70% of the respondents to be cooperative and thoughtful and rated the quality of interviews as high. Respondents understood and followed instructions. When asked about the tasks involved in the interview, respondents frequently accurately summarized the instructions that they were given. Further, 83% of respondents reported that the measurement process did not change their opinions or feelings about various health states. This result is consistent with our a priori hypotheses that people have well developed preferences for health states that can be measured in well designed and executed face-to-face interviews.

More than 98% of respondents reported some attributes to be more important than others, but as a group respondents focussed on all of the HUI3 health attributes, including both physical and mental health attributes. Each of the eight attributes was reported as being important by a substantial (>ā€‰5%) proportion of respondents. A possible limitation of the debriefing is that asking if one or two attributes were more important, we may have encouraged respondents to provide a positive response thereby exaggerating the effect at the individual level but illuminating the variability in the importance of attributes at the group level. More generally, a limitation of the study is that how we asked the questions may have influenced the responses provided by the respondents.

That each of the eight attributes was regarded as important is evidence consistent with the content validity of the HUI3 health-status classification system and multi-attribute utility function. Further the lack of correspondence between the prevalence of disability by attributes and the importance attached to the attributes implies that the importance of attributes should not be assumed to be based on the degree of familiarity with the disability associated with that attribute, that is by the prevalence of disability for that attribute.

The results also show that respondents considered the impacts of disabilities in the health states described. As a group they reported considering a wide variety of impacts of the health states as important, including: their ability to look after themselves, their family life, the happiness of others, their ability to work at their current job and the effects on their leisure activities. The five impact factors listed reflect recommendations about the health impacts that should be reflected in preference scores ([13], p 109).

Three of the factors listed focussed on the impacts of the health state for the respondent (ability to look after self; ability to work; leisure activities); two focussed on impacts on others (family life; happiness of others). An implication is that their preference scores reflect consequences both for self and for others. This in turn has implications for the interpretation of scores and the issue of accounting for spillover effects in cost-utility analyses [7, 20, 26].

Gold et al. [13] argue that role function should be included in health-status classification systems. The ā€œwithin the skinā€ approach embodied in the HUI3 has been criticized because social role function is not explicitly included in the health-status classification system [5]. However, respondents to the preference survey appear to have taken social roles into account when providing preference scores. That the happiness of others and family life were regarded as important factors might imply that respondents did take the effects of health states on social interaction into account in providing scores.

Related evidence on this issue is found in Hays et al. [14] who report results from a linear equating algorithm to map responses from PROMIS Global Health Items to overall HUI3 scores. The PROMIS scales used in the analyses included physical functioning, depressive symptoms, pain interference, ability to participate in social roles and activities, anxiety, and pain rating. In the results reported by Hays and colleagues the PROMIS domains that were predictive of overall HUI3 scores and included in the mapping algorithm are physical functioning, pain interference, anxiety, depressive symptoms, and ability to participate in social roles and activities. It is noteworthy that although social activities are not an HUI3 attribute, the ability to participate in social roles and activities is an important predictor of overall HUI3 scores. One interpretation of this result is that social activities in HUI3 are distal, not proximal, such that deficits in HUI3 attributes (proximal effects, for instance, of disabilities in hearing or cognition) imply restrictions on the ability to participate in social roles (distal) that influence the score for the HUI3 health state ā€” that respondents took these consequences into account in providing the scores for health states. Further, there is overlap between the PROMIS domains that predict HUI3 scores and the attributes and factors that were regarded as important by the respondents, providing evidence of the construct validity of the HUI3 system.

With respect to the potential double-counting productivity issues discussed in the Introduction, a majority of respondents indicated that they took their ability to work at their current job into account in providing preference scores for health states. This does not necessarily imply that respondents ignored the interview script instructions that their income would be the same regardless of the health state. That the majority of respondents took the ability to work into account could imply that people value the capacity to engage in meaningful activities and does not necessarily imply that respondents are incorporating the monetary effects of labor force participation in the utility scores they provide. The extent to which respondents took the monetary effects of work into account, as opposed to the health-related quality of life effects, is unknown.

In general, the results underscore the importance of the substantial variability in preferences for health states and, typically, little of this variability is systematically associated with socio-demographic characteristics [9]. Respondents differed with respect to which attributes were the most important and which impact factors were most important, emphasizing the need for broadly representative samples of respondents. The results of the statistical tests indicate that while there is heterogeneity in preferences, not all attributes or all impacts are of equal importance. Responses to the questions indicate that respondents thought carefully about the implications of the health states for their ability to engage in life.

The results are also consistent with a related theme, Kaplanā€™s Ziggy Theorem [15]. Kaplan argues that health is valuable because it provides the capacity to perform activities that are valued. Good health enables one to participate in valued activities.

Further, the results are consistent with observations by Dr. James Fries in discussing the fears of growing old expressed by patients. Dr. Fries notes that individuals dread pain, fear senility and the loss of memory, and fear being dependent on others ([8], p 411). Indeed these observations resonate with the views expressed by the HUI3 respondents.

This study provides important new evidence from a random sample of the general population about the factors that respondents consider in making preference judgments about health states. Collectively, respondents considered all eight HUI3 attributes to be important in defining health status (content validity), although there is considerable variability among individuals about which specific attributes are the most important. Similarly, there was heterogeneity in the impact factors considered to be the most important. The item with the highest level of importance was the ability to look after oneself. Finally including questions about what factors were important to respondents in providing their preference responses provides important information that is useful in interpreting preference scores for HUI3 health states.

Two issues relevant to all, or virtually all, generic preference-based measure raised by an anonymous reviewer (thank you) warrant discussion. The reviewer notes that the preference scores used to estimate multi-attribute utility functions are typically drawn from random samples of the general population and thus, for the most part, are provided by relatively healthy individuals. It is of course possible that those with more experience with ill health would rate health states differently than would a group of mainly healthy respondents. To the best of our knowledge, this potential limitation applies to all of the major generic preference-based measures. The issue warrants further research.

The reviewer also wonders if the values attached to health states change over time. Of course, the nature of our study design, a cross-sectional survey at a point in time, provides no evidence on the stability or lack of stability in preferences for health states over time. Again this potential limitation applies to all of the generic preference-based measures and has implications for the research agenda.

Conclusions

People have well developed preferences for HUI3 health states that can be measured in well designed and executed face-to-face interviews. All HUI3 attributes are important. Attribute-specific disability rates should not be considered as a substitute for attribute importance ratings. Social interaction effects were taken into account by respondents when providing preference scores for HUI3 ā€œwithin-the-skinā€ health states.

The explicit description of instructions given to respondents about what to, and what not to, consider in providing their preference scores for health states is important information. An implication is that information about the instructions should be incorporated into standard reporting practices for utility assessment studies in general, and studies reporting on the estimation of multi-attribute utility functions in particular. Similarly, the description of the process of any debriefing and reporting of the results from the debriefing also add value and should also be incorporated into reporting standards. Finally, we encourage further research, including formal qualitative and quantitative research on the role of the instructions given to respondents.

Abbreviations

CB:

Chance Board

FT:

Feeling Thermometer

HRQL:

Health-related quality of life

HUI2:

Health Utilities Index Mark 2

HUI3:

Health Utilities Index Mark 3

QALYs:

Quality-adjusted life years

QWB:

Quality of Well Being Scale

SF-6D:

Short-Form 6D

References

  1. Brazier J, Roberts J, Deverill M. The estimation of a preference-based measure of health status from the SF-36. J Health Econ. 2002;21(2):271ā€“92.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  2. Devlin NJ, Hansen P, Selai C. Understanding health state valuations: a qualitative analysis of respondentsā€™ comments. Qual Life Res. 2004;13(7):1265ā€“77.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  3. Dolan P. Modeling valuations for EuroQol health states. Med Care. 1997;35(11):1095ā€“108.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  CASĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  4. Feeny D, Furlong W, Torrance GW, Goldsmith CH, Zhu Z, DePauw S, Denton M, Boyle M. Multi-attribute and single-attribute utility functions for the health utilities index mark 3 system. Med Care. 2002;40(2):113ā€“28.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  5. Feeny D, Furlong W, Boyle M, Torrance GW. Multi-attribute health status classification systems: health utilities index. PharmacoEconomics. 1995;7(6):490ā€“502.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  CASĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  6. Feeny D. ā€œPreference-Based Measures: Utility and Quality-Adjusted Life Years,ā€ Chapter 6.2. In: Fayers P, Hays R, editors. Assessing quality of life in clinical trials. Second ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2005. p. 405ā€“29.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  7. Feeny D, Krahn M, Prosser LA, Salomon JA. ā€œValuing Health Outcomes,ā€ Chapter 7. In: Neumann PJ, Sanders GD, Russell LB, Siegel JE, Ganiats TG, editors. Cost-effectiveness in health and medicine. Second ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 2017. p. 167ā€“200.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  8. Fries, James F, ā€œThe Compression of Morbidity.ā€ Milbank Memorial Fund, Vol. 61, No. 3, Special Issue, Summer, 1983, pp 397ā€“419.

  9. Furlong, William John, ā€œVariability of Utility Scores for Health States Among General Population Groups,ā€ Unpublished MSc. Thesis, McMaster University, August, 1996.

  10. Furlong, William, David Feeny, George W. Torrance, Ronald Barr, and John Horsman, ā€œGuide to design and development of health-state utility instrumentation.ā€ McMaster University Centre for Health Economics and Policy Analysis Working Paper No 90-9, 1990.

  11. Furlong, William, David Feeny, George W. Torrance, Charles Goldsmith, Sonja DePauw, Zenglong Zhu, Margaret Denton, and Michael Boyle, ā€œMultiplicative multi-attribute utility function for the health utilities index mark 3 (HUI3) system: a technical report,ā€ McMaster University Centre for Health Economics and Policy Analysis Working Paper No 98-11, 1998.

  12. Furlong William J., David H. Feeny, George W. Torrance, and Ronald D. Barr, ā€œThe Health Utilities Index (HUI) System for Assessing Health-Related Quality of Life in Clinical Studies.ā€ Annals of Medicine, Vol. 33, No. 5, 2001, pp 375ā€“384.

  13. Gold MR, Patrick DL, Torrance GW, Fryback DG, Hadorn DC, Kamlet MS, Daniels N, Weinstein MC. ā€œIdentifying and Valuing Outcomes,ā€ Chapter 4. In: Gold MR, Siegel JE, Russell LB, Weinstein MC, editors. Cost-Effectiveness in Health and Medicine. New York: Oxford University Press; 1996. p. 82ā€“134.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  14. Hays, Ron D., Dennis Revicki, David Feeny, Peter Fayers, Karen I. Spritzer, and David Cella, ā€œPredicting HUI-3 Scores from PROMIS Global Health Items and PROMIS-29 Profile Measureā€. Pharmacoeconomics, Vol. 34, No. 10, 2016, pp 1015ā€“1022; published online 26 April 2016.

  15. Kaplan RM. The Ziggy theorem: toward an outcomes-focused Health Psychology. Health Psychology. 1994;13(6):451ā€“60.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  CASĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  16. Kaplan RM, Anderson JP. ā€œThe General Health Policy Model: An Integrated Approach,ā€ Chapter 32. In: Spilker B, editor. Quality of Life and Pharmacoeconomics in Clinical Trials. Second ed. Philadelphiar: Lippincott-Raven Press; 1996. p. 309ā€“22.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  17. Krol M, Brouwer W, Sendi P. Productivity costs in health-state valuations. Does explicit instruction matter? Pharmacoeconomics. 2006;24(4):401ā€“14.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  18. McDowell I, Newell C. Measuring Health: A guide to rating scales and questionnaires. Second ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 1996.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  19. Neumann PJ, Sanders GD, Russell LB, Siegel JE, Ganiats TG, editors. Cost-Effectiveness in Health and Medicine. Second ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 2017.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  20. Prosser LA, Lamarand K, Gebremariam A, Wittenberg E. Measuring family HRQoL spillover effects using direct health utility assessment. Med Decis Mak. January, 2015;35(1):81ā€“93.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  21. Rabin, Rosalind, and Frank de Charro, EQ-5D: A Measure of Health Status from the EuroQol Group. Annals of Medicine, Vol. 33, No. 5, July, 2001, pp 337ā€“343.

  22. Sculpher MJ, Oā€™Brien BJ. Income effects of reduced health and health effects of reduced income: implications for health-state valuation. Med Decis Mak. April-June, 2000;20(2):207ā€“15.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  23. Shiroiwa T, Fukuda T, Ikeda S, Shimozuma K. QALY and productivity losses: empirical evidence for ā€˜double countingā€™. Value Health. June, 2013;16(4):581ā€“7.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  24. Stalmeier PF, Goldstein MMK, Holmes AM, Lenert L, Miyamoto J, Stiggelbout AM, Torrance GW, Tsevat J. What Should Be Reported in a Methods Section on Utility Assessment? Medical Decision Making. 2001;21(3):200ā€“7.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  CASĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  25. Statistics Canada. Housing, Family and Social Statistics Division. General Social Survey Analysis Series. Health Status of Canadians: Report of the 1991 General Social Survey. Catalogue No. 11-612E, No. 8. ISBN 0ā€“660-15392E, No. 8. Ottawa, Canada 1994.

  26. Tilford JM, Payakachat N. Progress in measuring family spillover effects for economic evaluations. Expert Review of Pharmacoeonomics & Outcomes Research. 2015;15(2):195ā€“8.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  27. Tilling C, Krol M, Tsuchiya A, Brazier J, Brouwer W. In or out? Income losses in health state valuations: a review. Value Health. March-April, 2010;13(2):298ā€“305.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  28. Torrance GW, Feeny DH, Furlong WJ, Barr RD, Zhang Y, Wang Q. Multi-attribute preference functions for a comprehensive health status classification system: health utilities index mark 2. Med Care. 1996;34(7):702ā€“22.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  CASĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  29. Xie, Feng, A. Simon Pickard, Paul Krabbe, Dennis Revicki, Rosalie Viney, Nancy Devlin, and David Feeny, ā€œA checklist for reporting valuation studies of multi-attribute utility-based instruments (CREATE).ā€ PharmacoEconomics, Vol. 33, No. 8, August, pp 867ā€“877; published online May 31, 2015.

Download references

Acknowledgments

Many people contributed to the research project from which this study is derived including Karen Barber, Michael Boyle, Shelley Chambers, Margaret Denton, Sonja De Pauw, Aurelie Dumas, Charles H. Goldsmith, Ralph Keeney, Saba Safdar, David Streiner, Marilyn Swinton, Qinan Wang, Sharon Webb, the professional interviewers, and the survey respondents. An earlier version of this work was presented at the 23rd Annual Meeting of the International Society for Quality of Life Research (ISOQOL) in Copenhagen, 19-22 October 2017.

Funding

The authors acknowledge the financial support of the Ontario Ministry of Health (Grant 04020) and funding from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council (Grant 10020) and the Arts Research Board of McMaster University. The funding bodies had no role in the design and execution of the study, interpretation of the results, and writing of the manuscript.

Availability of data and materials

Data sharing not applicable to this article. Please contact author for data request.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All three authors made substantial contributions to the conception and design of the study, collection of the data, analyses and interpretation of the data, writing and editing of the manuscript, have given final approval for the submission of the manuscript, and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David Feeny.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by the McMaster University Research Ethics Board. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Consent for publication

Not Applicable.

Competing interests

It should be noted that David Feeny, William Furlong, and George W. Torrance have a proprietary interest in Health Utilities Incorporated (HUInc.), Dundas, Ontario, Canada. HUInc. distributes copyrighted Health Utilities Index (HUI) materials and provides methodological advice on the use of the HUI.

Publisherā€™s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix

Appendix

Table 5 Health Status Classification System: Health Utilities Index Mark 3 (HUI3)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Feeny, D., Furlong, W. & Torrance, G.W. What were they thinking when providing preference measurements for generic health states? The evidence for HUI3. Health Qual Life Outcomes 16, 166 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12955-018-0993-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12955-018-0993-9

Keywords