Skip to main content

Self-esteem in patients with venous thromboembolism predicts time trade-off values for own health

Abstract

Background

The time trade-off (TTO) is a commonly used preference based method to assess health related values used in health economic analyses. Surprisingly little is known about the factors influencing the TTO. Since self-esteem is a predictor for health status measurements, and health status affects the TTO, we studied the relation between self-esteem and TTO values.

Methods

Data of 128 patients treated with vitamin K antagonists for venous thromboembolism on Short Form-36 (SF-36), Rosenberg self-esteem and patient characteristics were collected. TTO values were obtained for ‘current health’ and three chronic health states related to thrombosis, in face-to-face interviews with patients. Regression analyses were performed with the TTO as dependent variable. Analyses were performed in two groups; the complete sample, and traders only. Selected predictors were entered in four blocks: socio-demographic factors, medical-clinical factors, health status, and self-esteem.

Results

In the complete sample (N = 128), bivariate regression analysis showed that self-esteem explained 14% of the variance in TTO values for current health (p < .000, N = 117). In traders, multivariate regression analysis showed a significant relationship between self-esteem and TTO values for current health. Self-esteem increased the variance explained (R2) by 8.8%, from 28.1 to 36.9%, (p = 0.01; N = 57). For hypothetical health states, the effect of self-esteem was weaker and mostly absent after controlling for selected variables.

Conclusions

In patients willing to trade-off time, higher self-esteem was associated with higher TTO values for own current health. Self-esteem explained an appreciable proportion of the variance in TTO values in traders. For hypothetical health states such associations were weak or absent.

Introduction

Preference-based instruments express the value or desirability of health in a single score [1]. Commonly used preference-based instruments are the standard gamble, time trade-off (TTO), rating scale and discrete choice experiments [1,2,3,4] {Arons, 2013 #79}. The TTO is a widely used health valuation measure in a large variety of countries. It has been used to obtain tariffs for health economic decision making. The TTO involves individual judgements of health relative to death and perfect health. Respondents sacrifice hypothetical life years in good health to avoid living in a lesser health state [1]. Sacrificing more life years would be expected for more severe health states, and corresponds with lower TTO values. In general, respondents may value their own health, that is self-experienced health, or hypothetical health states.

Despite its popularity, surprisingly little is known about factors influencing TTO values. In other words, the construct validity of the TTO, that is whether and to what extent preferences for health differ according to characteristics of respondents, has not been systematically studied [5]. Such knowledge is important to interpret TTO values, e.g. how the TTO will function in various contexts. It is true that medical factors have been shown to affect TTO or TTO based tariffs [6,7,8,9,10,11,12,13]. However, for other patient characteristics, such as socioeconomic variables, only weak associations with TTO values were reported [14,15,16,17,18]. It was also found that such relations were stronger for patients willing to trade time [17, 19, 20].

This study examined self-esteem as a TTO-determinant for three reasons. First, self-esteem is a positive predictor of several health status measures [21,22,23,24,25,26]. As only weak positive relations between health status measures and the TTO have been reported, it is not evident that self-esteem affects TTO values [19, 27,28,29,30]. Nevertheless, a positive relation was expected. Second, this study tests Tsevat’s hypothesis that self-esteem is positively related to the TTO. His hypothesis was based on qualitative observations in patients infected with human immunodeficiency virus (HIV), reporting TTO values for their self-experienced health [31, 32]. Third, the TTO confronts patients with ‘dead’. A sociopsychological theory relevant to death thoughts is Terror Management Theory [31, 33, 34]. The theory implies that “individuals’ valuations change when confronted with death in such a way that the standards and values, through which the individual acquires meaning in life and value in him/herself, are strengthened” [35, 36]. Self-esteem figures strongly in Terror Management Theory and it was conjectured to be positively related to TTO values and behavior [35, 36].

Taking into account these considerations, our research questions are: in patients who experienced venous thromboembolism, (1) is self-esteem related to the TTO values for their own health and hypothetical health, and (2) what is the relation between TTO and self-esteem in the subgroup of patients willing to trade life years.

Methods

Patients

Three groups of eligible patients were recruited between October 2000 and June 2002: (a) newly diagnosed patients with a first or second episode of venous thromboembolism for whom treatment with vitamin K antagonists had been started; (b) patients who had experienced an episode of major bleeding during treatment with vitamin K antagonists in the previous year; (c) patients with a post-thrombotic syndrome diagnosed at least one year after an episode of deep vein thrombosis, who had been treated with vitamin K antagonist for at least three months. Excluded were patients younger than 18 years, patients with a cancer or other serious co-morbidity, as well as patients with an insufficient knowledge of the Dutch language [12, 37].

The present study was performed at three academic centres: The Academic Medical Centre in Amsterdam, the University Hospital of Groningen, and the University Hospital of Maastricht. In each center, the study had been approved by the institutional review board [12].

Procedures

Eligible patients were invited to join the study by a physician or research nurse who explained the purpose of the study and provided written information [12]. Written informed consent was obtained. Physicians gathered medical-clinical data during the consultation by means of the medical dossier and by asking their patients directly. Afterwards patients were handed a questionnaire, assessing quality of life with the SF-36 instrument, self-esteem, and socio-demographic variables. They were asked to return this questionnaire by mail within 2 weeks. Also, arrangements were made for an interview in which the TTO assessment was included. As we wanted patients to have experienced treatment with vitamin K antagonists at the time of the interview, patients with an episode of venous thromboembolism were interviewed approximately three months after inclusion. Patients who experienced a major bleeding event and patients with a post-thrombotic syndrome had already been treated with vitamin K antagonist, and were therefore contacted a few days after inclusion to schedule an interview appointment in the near future.

All patients were interviewed by one of four trained interviewers. For quality control, each interviewer audio taped three of the first ten interviews and received detailed feedback from the investigators. On average, the interview lasted 62 min (range 32–120) [12].

Measures

Socio-demographic variables

In the questionnaire, information was gathered regarding patients’ sex, age, marital status, employment status, education, religion and family history of thromboembolism.

Time trade-off (interview)

TTO questions were asked face to face for eight thrombosis related health states and ‘current health’ [12]. Health states were described by vignettes mentioning concomitant physical, social, and mental domains. Vignettes were developed by clinical experts in the field. The vignette for ‘current health’ simply asked patients to consider these three domains for the past week while valuing their own health. Life expectancies, to be used in the TTO, were determined using tables from the CBS-statistics in the Netherlands [38]. Five health states were temporary and not considered here. Here, we study ‘current health’, and three chronic health states namely ‘non-fatal haemorrhagic stroke’, ‘post-thrombotic symptoms’, and ‘no treatment with vitamin K antagonists’ [12]. The TTO question asked the patient to choose, using a series of choices, between the health state under consideration for their remaining life expectancy (t), and a variable amount of time in perfect health (x); both states are followed by death [37]. In the first two choices, the duration of time in perfect health was set equal to zero or remaining life expectancy, in random order. The bisection method was used to vary the time spent in perfect health until the patient reported indifference between the two options. The value for own current health was calculated by dividing x by t and multiplying it by 100. The TTO therefore ranged from 0 (representing death) and 100 (representing perfect health) [12, 37]. The research team decided not to elicit values worse than dead for ethical reasons, and because of the complicated nature of such questions.

Rosenberg self-esteem (questionnaire)

The Rosenberg self-esteem instrument assesses patients’ self-esteem using a questionnaire in which ten items are rated on a four-point Likert scale ranging from ‘strongly agree’ to ‘strongly disagree’. Examples of items are: “I feel that I’m a person of worth, at least on an equal plane with others” and “At times I think I am no good at all”. The scale was computed by rescaling the negative questions and then adding the ten scores. Scores were only computed when all ten items were answered. The total Self-esteem score ranges from 10 to 40, with higher values indicating higher self-esteem [39].

Short form health survey (SF-36 Questionnaire)

The SF-36 health survey is a standardized questionnaire used to assess patient health status across eight dimensions [40]. There is extensive evidence of the ability of these scores to describe the health differences between different patient groups and, more important for evaluation, their ability to detect health changes following intervention [41,42,43,44]. The SF-36 instrument measures eight domains of health: physical functioning, role limitations due to physical problems, bodily pain, general health perceptions, vitality, social functioning, role limitations due to emotional problems, and mental health. The validated Dutch version of the Medical Outcomes Study 36 item Short Form Health Survey (SF-36) was used [45]. The timeframe employed was the previous week. Each of the eight domains was computed if at least 50% of the questions in that category were answered. All scores were linearly converted to a 0–100 scale, with higher scores indicating better levels of health status [46, 47]. The Mental Component Summary score (MCS) and Physical Component Summary score (PCS) were computed following a standardized three step procedure [46, 47]. The mean and standard deviation of the U.S. population were used for transforming and computing summary scores [46, 47]. Patients’ SF-36 scores were compared with published age and sex-matched Dutch reference population norms that were computed identically [45].

Statistical analysis

All analyses were performed using SPSS 25.0 for windows. Prior to analysis we examined patient characteristics and primary outcome variables. In our study we sought to establish whether self-esteem is a predictor for TTO values. We performed statistical analyses in two selections of patients: (1) analyses in the complete sample, and (2) analyses in traders. Traders are respondents willing to trade-off time in the Time Trade-off task to gain better health. Non-traders are not willing to trade-off any time to gain better health, so their utility equals 1.

In general, non-traders should be included in the sample to estimate mean TTO values. However, our research question is about associations. As all TTO values in non-traders are equal to 100, whatever the value of the predictor, correlations of any predictor with the TTO value will be zero. Thus, larger proportions of non-traders in the sample will weaken or mask possible relations between predictors and the TTO [17, 19, 20]. For this reason, separate analyses in non-traders were done.

In the complete and traders’ samples, we examined bivariate relationship between self-esteem and TTO. For multivariate analyses, several blocks of variables were analyzed to select possible predictors. The blocks consisted of socio-demographic and medical-clinical factors, and the SF-36 summary scales. Each block of potential predictors was entered separately in multivariate regression analysis; predictors with p < 0.2 were selected for our final multivariate models. In the final multivariate models, a result with a p value lower than 0.05 was considered significant.

Results

The sample comprised 128 respondents of which 3 (2.3%) had missing TTO values and 46 (35.9%) were not willing to trade-off any time. Table 1 shows the patient characteristics of our study population: 46.8% was male with a mean age of 52.5 (sd = 15.64), 61 (54%) had paid work, 59 (48.4%) were religious, 83 (66.9%) were married or living together, and 59 (50.4%) had no comorbidities. Patient characteristics were comparable with the Dutch population [45]. A summary description of the main variables (TTO, MCS, PCS and self-esteem) is shown in Table 2. Respondents on average scored 48.61 (sd = 12.07) on the MCS scale and 39.64 (sd = 10.76) on the PCS scale. These scores were substantially lower than the scores for the MCS 52.1 (sd = 9.6) and the PCS 49.7 (sd = 9.3) in the Dutch reference population [20]. Mean self-esteem was 31.08 (sd = 5.50) and ranged from 15 to 40.

Table 1 Patient characteristics of complete sample and traders only
Table 2 Description of primary outcome variables of total study population and traders only

Results in the complete sample

In the complete sample, TTO values for ‘current health’ were negatively skewed with a mean and median value of 88.5 and 98.75 respectively, N = 124. 35.9% of the patients were non-traders. In bivariate analysis self-esteem was related to TTO values for ‘current health’ (r = 0.374, p < 0.000, N = 117) explaining 14% of the variance of TTO values. A multivariate model including the block-wise selected variables gender, comorbidities, and PCS, showed that adding self-esteem increased the explained variance by 7.6%, from 22.2 to 29.8%, p = 0.002, N = 97 (data not shown). Comorbidities was still significant in the full model at p = 0.003).

In the complete sample, TTO values for the hypothetical health state ‘non-fatal haemorrhagic stroke’ were strongly positively skewed with a mean and median value of 23.5 and 0, respectively, N = 123. 57% of the patients gave a value of zero (all-in traders). In bivariate analyses, self-esteem was not related to TTO values (r = 0.024, p = 0.79, N = 116), therefore further analyses were not considered.

In the complete sample, TTO values for the hypothetical health state ‘post-thrombotic symptoms’, were negatively skewed, with a mean and median, of 83,8, and 90,4, respectively (N = 124). 22.7% of the patients were non-traders. In bivariate analyses, self-esteem was not related to TTO values (r = 0.17, p = 0.07, N = 124), therefore further analyses were not considered.

In the complete sample, TTO values for the hypothetical health state ‘no treatment with vitamin K antagonists’, were negatively skewed with a mean and median value of 89.9 and 98.8, (N = 124). 39.8% of the patients were non-traders. In bivariate analyses, self-esteem was related to the TTO values for ‘no treatment with vitamin K antagonists’, (r = 0.236, p = 0.01, N = 117), explaining 5.7% of the variance. A multivariate model including the block-wise selected variables gender, ‘light to mild post thrombotic symptoms’ and ‘severe post thrombotic symptoms’ showed that adding self-esteem increased the explained variance explained by 5.2%, that is from 6.5% to 11.7%, p < 0.01, N = 117) (data not shown). The variables ‘mild to light, and ‘severe post thrombotic symptoms’ remained significant at p = 0.32, and p = 0.012 respectively.

Results in traders

In the traders group, TTO values for ‘current health were examined’. Three respondents had missing values, leaving 74 traders for analysis. In bivariate analyses, self-esteem was related to the TTO values for ‘current health’, (r = 0.427, p = 0.000, N = 74), explaining 18.2% of the variance in TTO-values. Selected predictors for the restricted model were ‘mild to light post thrombotic symptoms, ‘severe post thrombotic symptoms’, comorbidities, recent events, and PCS (see Table 3). The results of the restricted model are presented in the upper part of Table 4, explaining 28.1% of the variance. There were 57 respondents left due to attrition caused by missing data. The full model (N = 57, lower part of Table 4) showed that adding self-esteem increased the explained variance by 8.8%, that is from 28.1% without self-esteem, to 36.9% including self-esteem (p = 0.011). Severe post thrombotic symptoms and comorbidities were still significant in the full model at p = 0.079, and p = 0.009, respectively.

Table 3 In traders only, four separate multivariate linear regressions to select predictors for the TTO
Table 4 In traders only, multivariate linear regression model with selected predictors for TTO values

In the traders group, values for the hypothetical health state ‘non-fatal haemorrhagic stroke’ were examined. Instead of removing non-traders, all-in traders were removed because 57% of the respondents gave a value of zero. In bivariate analyses, self-esteem was unrelated related to the TTO values. Therefore further analyses were not considered.

In the traders group, values for the hypothetical health state ‘post-thrombotic symptoms’ were examined. In bivariate analyses, self-esteem was related to the TTO values (r = 0.211, p = 0.045, N = 91), Using the block wise selection procedure, only living status was selected. The explained variance in the restricted model without self-esteem was 4.6%, p = 0.024, N = 91). Adding self-esteem did not increase the amount of variance explained (increase = 3.7%, p = 0.067).

In the traders group, values for the hypothetical health state ‘no treatment with vitamin K antagonists’ were examined. In bivariate analyses, self-esteem was related to the TTO values (r = 0.281, p = 0.018, N = 70). Selected predictors were gender, working status, education, light and severe post thrombotic symptoms and comorbidity. The explained variance in the restricted model was 22% (N = 62). Adding self-esteem did not increase the amount of variance explained (increase = 4.5%, p = 0.075).

Summary of the results

In the complete and traders samples, TTO values for current health were moderately related to self-esteem, both in bivariate as well as in multivariate analysis controlling for selected variables. In the complete sample, for 2 out of 3 hypothetical health states, bivariate relations showed that self-esteem was weakly related TTO values; for one hypothetical health states, self-esteem significantly added 5.2% of the variance after controlling for selected variables. In traders, bivariate analyses again showed that self-esteem was weakly related to TTO values for 2 out of 3 hypothetical health states. The contribution of self-esteem was no longer significant after controlling for selected variables.

Discussion

We sought to establish whether self-esteem is a predictor of TTO values in order to investigate the construct validity of the TTO values. In our study, the sample consisted of 128 patients treated with vitamin K antagonists for venous thromboembolism. Among all respondents, adding self-esteem increased the explained variance by 7.6%. Among patients willing to trade time, after controlling for selected predictors, adding self-esteem increased the explained variance of TTO for ‘current health’ by 8.8%. For hypothetical health states, the effect of self-esteem was weaker and mostly absent after controlling for selected variables.

Interpretation

The findings for ‘current health’ are discussed first. The physical symptoms scale of the SF-36 (PCS) was related to TTO values for ‘current health’. This is reasonable since if SF-36 scores for one’s own health improve, one expects higher value for one’s current health. The relation between PSC and TTO current health values was found in the complete sample and in traders. These results replicate the findings in other studies reporting correlations between SF-36 and TTO values ranging from 0.01 to 0.60 [19, 27,28,29,30, 48]. When self-esteem was included, a sizeable amount of additional variance was explained: 7.6% in the complete same and 8.8% in traders. Thus our study establishes that self-esteem is an independent predictor of TTO values, even after controlling for the SF-36 health status measure. Our study confirms results of Mrus who found in veterans and in bivariate analyses, that an increase in self-esteem predicted standard gambles values for ‘current health’ [39, 49]. Our study confirms an earlier hypothesis of Tsevat that self-esteem is a predictor of the TTO [31, 32]. His hypothesis was based on qualitative observations in patients infected with human immunodeficiency virus (HIV), valuing their self-experienced health whereas our quantitative results are obtained in an observational study. Our results suggests that raising self-esteem is a relevant target to address in low-self-esteem populations {Rodriguez-Ayllon, 2019 #73}{Gow, 2020 #74}{Siette, 2017 #76}.

For the hypothetical states, self-esteem did not significantly affect the TTO-values in all but one analysis, that is for the hypothetical health state ‘no treatment with vitamin K antagonists’, in the complete sample. The results suggest that the effect for hypothetical states is weaker. We offer two possible explanations. First. we expected self-esteem to be important while valuing one’s own health, but less so while valuing hypothetical health states. This was based on the assumption that valuing one’s own health generates a self-focused condition. In such a condition, thoughts occur such as ‘who am I’, or ‘what is important to me’, thoughts which touch on self-esteem [50]. We assume that such a self-focused condition is less strong in hypothetical health states, where respondents are more concerned with judging attributes and their levels. A second explanation may be a lack of power given the weak relations between self-esteem and TTO values. Post-hoc power analyses showed that the achieved power for the hypothetical health states in traders was around 52%.

Implications for the hypothesized role of self-esteem and Terror Management Theory

TTO arguably engenders thoughts of death and Terror Management Theory is about death thoughts. Therefore, a connection between TTO values and Terror Management Theory was suspected. In Terror Management Theory, self-esteem is a core concept and an important defense against death thoughts. This was one of the reasons to include self-esteem in the current study. The relation between cultural values and TTO values is explained in detail in recent work [35]. The present results support the expected relation between self-esteem and TTO values, based on this framework [35].

Consequences for health technology assessment agencies

To recognise the role of self-esteem in TTO values may facilitate sharing the interpretation of TTO values between the outcomes research community and healthcare decision makers, such as health technology assessment agencies. However, their decisions are usually, but not always, based on evaluations of health from the societal perspective. These are determined using societal valuations of hypothetical multi-attribute health states. For such an application, a small impact of self-esteem is expected given the weaker relations between self-esteem and TTO found here for hypothetical health states. For agencies that do indeed use values for current or self-experienced health, it is important to know that TTO values are not only affected by medical or psychological factors, but that self-esteem may play an important role.

Directions for further research

  1. 1.

    In future studies, employing the TTO, the present findings can be tested in other populations by including self-esteem as a predictor.

  2. 2.

    In future studies, one may confirm whether the relation between self-esteem for TTO values is indeed stronger for current health as compared to hypothetical health states.

  3. 3.

    Terror Management Theory contains many cultural values, e.g. being patriotic, or desire for having children. Such cultural values may be tested for their association with TTO values. A large number of such values is available {Burke, 2010 #78}. Such studies may serve to strengthen the link between Terror Management Theory and TTO values.

Limitations and strengths

A limitation is that the proportion of non-traders in our study population (35.9%) was higher than expected. The high prevalence of non-traders could be reason to question the quality of the interviews and validity of the TTO elicitation. However, the prevalence of non-traders in our sample is actually lower than the average proportion of 57% estimated in a review in other patient populations [28]. A second objection is that TTO values were skewed. However, in traders, the residuals of the regression models were normally distributed, justifying their use in traders. Another limitation is the time differences between the measurement of self-esteem at baseline and the TTO-interview, ranging from two weeks to 3 months. During this period, patients’ self-esteem status may have changed reducing possible relations. Nevertheless, despite these time differences, reliable associations were found.

A strength of this study is that the TTO values for current health were obtained in carefully conducted face to face interviews by trained interviewers. Another strength is the wide range in TTO values and self-esteem scores in facilitating the detection of possible relations.

Conclusion

The present study provides new and relevant information on the TTO method. Traders with higher self-esteem scores gave higher TTO values for their current health, even after controlling for socio-demographic variables, health status, and medical-clinical variables. This result may seem unsurprising as self-esteem is associated with health status, albeit weakly to moderately. Therefore, it was not clear in advance that self-esteem predicts the TTO. The relation between self-esteem and TTO values for hypothetical health states were weaker or absent. In general, health status, clinical variables and demographic variables can account for only a fraction of the variance in multivariable models of the TTO [15, 19, 31]. In this light, the additional 8.8% of variance explained by self-esteem found in traders may certainly be considered sizeable.

Availability of data materials

The datasets used in the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

HRQoL:

Health related quality of life

QOL:

Quality of life

TTO:

Time trade-off

SF-36:

Medical Outcomes Study Short Form-36 Questionnaire

MCS:

Mental Component Summary Scale

PCS:

Physical Component Summary Scale

PTS:

Post Thrombotic Syndrome

References

  1. Torrance GW, Thomas WH, Sackett DL. A utility maximization model for evaluation of health care programs. Health Serv Res. 1972;7:118–33.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Robinson A, Dolan P, Williams A. Valuing health status using VAS and TTO: what lies behind the numbers? Soc Sci Med. 1997;45:1289–97.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Robinson A, Loomes G, Jones-Lee M. Visual analog scales, standard gambles, and relative risk aversion. Med Decis Making. 2001;21:17–27.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Norman R, Craig BM, Hansen P, et al. Issues in the design of discrete choice experiments. Patient. 2019;12:281–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Dolan P, Roberts J. To what extent can we explain time trade-off values from other information about respondents? Soc Sci Med. 2002;54:919–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Longworth L, Yang Y, Young T, et al. Use of generic and condition-specific measures of health-related quality of life in NICE decision-making: a systematic review, statistical modelling and survey. Health Technol Assess. 2014;18:1–224.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Lopez-Jimenez F, Goldman L, Orav EJ, et al. Health values before and after pacemaker implantation. Am Heart J. 2002;144:687–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Saeed YA, Phoon A, Bielecki JM, et al. A systematic review and meta-analysis of health utilities in patients with chronic hepatitis C. Value Health. 2020;23:127–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Sullivan PW, Lawrence WF, Ghushchyan V. A national catalog of preference-based scores for chronic conditions in the United States. Med Care. 2005;43:736–49.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Tengs TO, Wallace A. One thousand health-related quality-of-life estimates. Med Care. 2000;38:583–637.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Tengs TO, Lin TH. A meta-analysis of utility estimates for HIV/AIDS. Med Decis Making. 2002;22:475–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Locadia M, Bossuyt PM, Stalmeier PF, et al. Treatment of venous thromboembolism with vitamin K antagonists: patients’ health state valuations and treatment preferences. Thromb Haemost. 2004;92:1336–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Fryback DG, Dasbach EJ, Klein R, et al. The Beaver Dam Health Outcomes Study: initial catalog of health-state quality factors. Med Decis Making. 1993;13:89–102.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Devlin NJ, Tsuchiya A, Buckingham K, et al. A uniform time trade off method for states better and worse than dead: feasibility study of the “lead time” approach. Health Econ. 2011;20:348–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. van Nooten F, Busschbach J, van Agthoven M, et al. What should we know about the person behind a TTO? Eur J Health Econ HEPAC Health Econ Prev Care. 2018;19:1207–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. van Nooten FE, van Exel NJ, Koolman X, et al. “Married with children” the influence of significant others in TTO exercises. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2015;13:94.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Handler RM, Hynes LM, Nease RF Jr. Effect of locus of control and consideration of future consequences on time tradeoff utilities for current health. Qual Life Res. 1997;6:54–60.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Jakubczyk M, Golicki D, Niewada M. The impact of a belief in life after death on health-state preferences: true difference or artifact? Qual Life Res. 2016;25:2997–3008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Bult JR, Hunink MG, Tsevat J, et al. Heterogeneity in the relationship between the time tradeoff and Short Form-36 for HIV-infected and primary care patients. Med Care. 1998;36:523–32.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Buitinga L, Braakman-Jansen LM, Taal E, et al. Construct validity of the interview time trade-off and computer time trade-off in patients with rheumatoid arthritis: a cross-sectional observational pilot study. BMC Musculoskelet Disord. 2012;13:112.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Eiser C, Morse R. Quality-of-life measures in chronic diseases of childhood. Health Technol Assess. 2001;5:1–157.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. De Boer M, Grootenhuis M, Derkx B, et al. Health-related quality of life and psychosocial functioning of adolescents with inflammatory bowel disease. Inflamm Bowel Dis. 2005;11:400–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Gureje O, Harvey C, Herrman H. Self-esteem in patients who have recovered from psychosis: profile and relationship to quality of life. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2004;38:334–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Han K, Lee P, Lee S, et al. Factors influencing quality of life in people with chronic illness in Korea. J Nurs Scholarsh. 2003;35:139–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Ben-Shlomo Y, Camfield L, Warner T. What are the determinants of quality of life in people with cervical dystonia? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2002;72:608–14.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Pedro LW. Quality of life for long-term survivors of cancer: influencing variables. Cancer Nurs. 2001;24:1–11.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Revicki DA, Kaplan RM. Relationship between psychometric and utility-based approaches to the measurement of health-related quality of life. Qual Life Res. 1993;2:477–87.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Arnesen T, Trommald M. Roughly right or precisely wrong? Systematic review of quality-of-life weights elicited with the time trade-off method. J Health Serv Res Policy. 2004;9:43–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Perez DJ, McGee R, Campbell AV, et al. A comparison of time trade-off and quality of life measures in patients with advanced cancer. Qual Life Res. 1997;6:133–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Albrecht GL, Devlieger PJ. The disability paradox: high quality of life against all odds. Soc Sci Med. 1999;48:977–88.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Tsevat J. What do utilities measure? Med Care. 2000;38:Iil60–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Tsevat J. SMDM presidential address: Hearsay or heresy—are health decision scientists too left brained? Med Decis Making. 2003;23:83–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Greenberg J, Pyszczynski T, Solomon S, et al. Role of consciousness and accessibility of death-related thoughts in mortality salience effects. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;67:627–37.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Greenberg J, Pyszczynski T, Solomon S. The causes and consequences of the need for self-esteem: a terror management theory. In: Baumeister RF, editor. Public self and private self. New York: Springer; 1986. p. 189–212.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  35. Stalmeier PFM, Roudijk B. Prolonging life: terror management theory to explain time trade-off values: a concept study (submitted).

  36. Wiggers LC, Stalmeier PF, Oort FJ, et al. Do patients’ preferences predict smoking cessation? Prev Med. 2005;41:667–75.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Locadia M, Stalmeier PF, Oort FJ, et al. A comparison of 3 valuation methods for temporary health states in patients treated with oral anticoagulants. Med Decis Making. 2004;24:625–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Central Bureau for Statistics, the Netherlands.

  39. Rosenberg M. Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 1965.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  40. Ware J, Snow K, Kosinski M, et al. SF 36 Health Survey: manual and interpretation guide. Boston: The Health Institute: New England Medical Centre. 1993.

  41. Brazier J, Roberts J, Deverill M. The estimation of a preference-based measure of health from the SF-36. J Health Econ. 2002;21:271–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Brazier JE, Harper R, Jones NM, et al. Validating the SF-36 health survey questionnaire: new outcome measure for primary care. BMJ. 1992;305:160–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Garratt AM, Ruta DA, Abdalla MI, et al. The SF36 health survey questionnaire: an outcome measure suitable for routine use within the NHS? BMJ. 1993;306:1440–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Ruta DA, Abdalla MI, Garratt AM, et al. SF 36 health survey questionnaire, I: reliability in two patient based studies. Qual Health Care. 1994;3:180–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Aaronson NK, Muller M, Cohen PD, et al. Translation, validation, and norming of the Dutch language version of the SF-36 Health Survey in community and chronic disease populations. J Clin Epidemiol. 1998;51:1055–68.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Ware J, Kosinski M. Norm-based scoring of SF-36 PCS&MCS. SF-36 physical and mental health summary scales : a manual for users of Version 1. Lincoln, RI: QualityMetric, ©1994, 2001, 2002 printing; 2009. 32.

  47. Ware JE, Kosinski M. Interpreting SF-36 summary health measures: a response. Qual Life Res. 2001;10:405–13.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Tsevat J, Solzan JG, Kuntz KM, et al. Health values of patients infected with human immunodeficiency virus: relationship to mental health and physical functioning. Med Care. 1996;34:44–57.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Mrus JM, Leonard AC, Yi MS, et al. Health-related quality of life in veterans and nonveterans with HIV/AIDS. J Gen Intern Med. 2006;21(Suppl 5):S39-47.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Verplanken B, Holland RW. Motivated decision making: effects of activation and self-centrality of values on choices and behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2002;82:434–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Permission to use the data was granted by the project leader Prof. MH Prins, MD , PhD, now at MUMC in Maastricht, the Netherlands.

Funding

We thank the Netherlands Heart Foundation for funding the main study (NHF-99-172), of which this study is a secondary analysis. The publication of study results was not contingent on the sponsor’s approval of this manuscript, nor was there any sponsor censorship of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

PFMS and EEV contributed equally to all aspects of this study. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Peep F. M. Stalmeier.

Ethics declarations

Ethical approval and consent to participate

Ethical approval was waived.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

No financial and non-financial competing interests need to be declared.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Stalmeier, P.F.M., Volmeijer, E.E. Self-esteem in patients with venous thromboembolism predicts time trade-off values for own health. Health Qual Life Outcomes 20, 41 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12955-022-01947-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12955-022-01947-5

Keywords

JEL Classification