Skip to main content

Combined effects of education level and perceived social class on self-rated health and life satisfaction: Results of Korean labor and income panel study wave 8-wave 15

Abstract

Background

To examine the combined effects of education level and perceived social class on self-rated health and life satisfaction in South Korea.

Methods

We used data drawn from the 8 to 15th wave of the Korean Labor and Income Panel Study (KLIPS). Using wave 8 at baseline, data included 11,175 individuals. We performed a longitudinal analysis at baseline estimating the prevalence of self-rated health and life satisfaction among individuals by education level (high, middle, and low education level) and perceived social class (high, middle, and low social class).

Results

For self-rated health, odds ratio (OR) of individuals with low education and low perceived social class was 0.604 times lower (95 % CI: 0.555–0.656) and the OR of individuals with low education and middle perceived social class was 0.853 time lower (95 % CI: 0.790–0.922) when compared to individuals with high education and high perceived social class. For life satisfaction, OR of individuals with low education and low perceived social class was 0.068 times lower (95 % CI: 0.063–0.074) and the OR of individuals with middle education and middle perceived social class was 0.235 time lower (95 % CI: 0.221–0.251) compared to individuals with high education and high perceived social class.

Conclusions

This study shows that the combined effects of education level and perceived social class associated with self-rated health and life satisfaction. Our study suggests increasing education level and perceived social class. Additionally, it will be important to develop multi-dimensional measurement tools including education level and subjective social class.

Background

A number of social scientists have observed that a positive relationship exists between socioeconomic status (SES) indicators and health outcomes [1, 2]. Social, demographic, economic, and behavioural risk factors play an integral part in shaping one’s health and life satisfaction. These factors known as ‘social determinants of health’ do not exist in isolation from one another, but combine to determine the health of individuals, communities, and populations [3, 4]. House [5] presents a theoretical model linking social determinants with health. Social structural factors, such as sociodemographic and socioeconomic status, are considered underlying determinants of health. These factors are hypothesised to influence health primarily through their effects on proximate determinants such as psychosocial and behavioural risk factors. Accordingly, sociodemographic factors (e.g. sex, age, race, and nativity) interact with socioeconomic factors and influence one’s exposure to social stressors, health practices and behaviours, access to medical care and insurance, and ultimately health. However, more recently investigators have focused on perceived social class as a predictor of health. Multiple studies demonstrate that perceived social class continues to predict health once traditional measures of SES, like income and education, have been taken into account [6, 7].

One study showed that perceived social class was more consistently and strongly related to health-related factors than traditional measures of SES, such as income and education [8]. Another study showed that perceived social class can predict change in health status [7]. However, there is little agreement on which indicators should be used for examining the association between SES and health outcome. As a result, recent studies [9, 10] suggest the use of a combined effect of SES on health outcome. In addition, although the SES gradient in health has been studied widely, how social class causes poorer health is not clear. This may be, in part, because social class has been defined as SES, which is an external, purely objective measure that does not account for subjective, internalised perceived social class.

One previous study [11] suggested that these perceptions of relative ranking may be more important determinants of health than traditional measures, such as level of education and income, which assess material resources [12]. However, many previous studies used both objective and perceived social class indicators. In addition, despite the vast literature on the effect of SES on health, little is known about people’s perceptions of their placement in the social hierarchy, what determines these perceptions, or how these perceptions relate to health.

Although routine analysis using conceptually coherent and consistent measures of social class remains rare, income, deprivation, wealth, and education have typically been used to measure social class [9]. There have been many studies showing positive associations between traditional measures (i.e. education) and health outcomes, and between perceived social class and health outcomes.

Because education is easy to measure and applicable to persons, level of education is among the most widely used indicators of socioeconomic status in US public health research. Education level among adults who have completed their schooling, for example, is not affected by occurrence of serious illness, which can force individuals to work at jobs below the level of their normal occupations or otherwise cause their income to decline. Selection of education as a practical measure of socioeconomic position for the 1989 revision of the US standard death certificate was based on these considerations [13].

Especially, education level as traditional measures is a strong and consistent predictor of health outcomes [14]. Self-rated health has been found to be a good predictor of morbidity and mortality, and traditional measures of SES (e.g. education) are associated with self-rated health [15]. Those factors are related to life satisfaction. However, no study to date has examined the combined effect of education and perceived social class on self-rated health and life satisfaction. Therefore, in this study we investigated the size of combined effects of education level and perceived social class on self-rated health and life satisfaction in South Korea.

Methods

Data were drawn from the 8 to 15th wave of the Korean Labor and Income Panel Study (KLIPS) that included various social classes. The survey passed an ethical review process by Statistics Korea, a central government organization for statistics. This study did not require an ethical review as the KLIPS dataset was publicly open and lacked information for individual identification.

The KLIPS is a longitudinal study of a representative sample of Korean households and individuals living in urban areas. The sampling method used at baseline was a two-stage stratified cluster sampling [16]. Households were selected after selecting the region. However, when considering the relatively low population ratio in rural regions of Korea (i.e. gun, eup, and myeon areas: the population ratio of gun, eup, and myeon areas was analysed as 18.0 % in the 2010 Population Census), it can be said that this study holds significant value as representative data for Korea. To give an equal probability of being sampled, weights were assigned to each respondent, enabling the results to represent the entire Korean population. This weighting method guarantees unbiased point estimates of population parameters for the entire population and its subsets.

As a type of study that possesses both the strengths of cross-sectional data and time series data, the KLIPS was constructed by repeatedly surveying the identical content for the same respondents every year. Thus, all variables surveyed by the KLIPS were repeatedly measured from the 1st wave to the 15th wave to collect observation cases at multiple points in time. Five thousand households in an urban region were selected, excluding households within a rural region. All household members residing in the relevant households form the panel sample, with repeated observations on the selected sample once a year every year.

Since 1998, the KLIPS has involved longitudinal research with annual assessments, tracing factors associated with labour movement and economic activities as well as income, expenditures, education, job training, and social activities of individuals. The original sample of the KLIPS (5,000 households) were sampled by two-stage stratified clustering, which first involved selection of the enumeration districts then selection of the households. Following cases can be added to KLIPS after survey started: 1) people in selected households who became 15 years-old, 2) people who were targeted for the survey but had not responded and became able to complete the survey, and 3) people who were over 15 years old that were newly added from selected households.

We only included participants from the 8 to 15th wave of KLIPS that had all the information needed for the study. Among the Wave 8 data consisting of 11,580 individuals, we excluded 405 individuals without information. Thus, the Wave 8 data included 11,175 individuals. The remaining Waves and number of individuals included are as follows: 11,487 of 11,756 in Wave 9, 11,487 of 11,855 in Wave 10, 11,606 of 11,734 in Wave 11, 14,355 of 14,489 in Wave 12, 14,094 of 14,118 in Wave 13, 13,778 of 13,900 in Wave 14, and 13,866 of 14,000 in Wave 15.

Study variables

Dependent variables

Self-rated health was measured by the question: ‘How do you rate your health compared with your age peers?’ The item was rated 1 (Very bad) to 5 (Very good). The reliability of the self-rated health question is found to be as good as or even better than that of most of the more specific questions on health [17]. Moreover, the question is closely related to health outcomes [17].

Life satisfaction was measured by the question: ‘How satisfied are you at present with your life?’ The item was rated 1 (very bad) to 5 (very good). Single-item measures of life satisfaction have previously been effectively used [18]. For both dependent variables, the response of ‘very bad’ or ‘bad’ indicated ‘bad’, and the response of ‘normal’ indicated ‘normal’, the response of ‘good’, or ‘very good’ indicated ‘good’.

Independent variables

Education level

Education level was categorised into three groups: middle school or lower (Low), high school (Middle), and college or higher (High).

Perceived social class

Perceived social class was measured by asking the respondents to assess their social class based on the following question: ‘Where do you think you belong upon consideration of income, job, education level, and assets’. The item was rated 1 (Very Low) to 6 (Very High). The response of ‘very low’ or ‘low’ indicated ‘Low’. The response of ‘middle-low’ or ‘middle-high’ indicated ‘Middle’. The response of ‘high’ or ‘very high’ indicated ‘High’.

Combined effects of education level and perceived social class

Combined effects represent the difference between the education level and perceived social class. We categorised the nine groups as follows: 1) high education and low perceived social class, 2) high education and middle perceived social class, 3) high education and high perceived social class, 4) middle education and low perceived social class, 5) middle education and middle perceived social class, 6) middle education and high perceived social class, 7) low education and low perceived social class, 8) low education and middle perceived social class, and 9) low education and high perceived social class. Thus, we analysed combined effects of education level and perceived social class on self-rated health and life satisfaction.

Control variables

Age was divided into six categories: ≤29, 30–39, 40–49, 50–59, 60–69, and ≥70 years. Residential regions were categorised as Capital (Seoul), big city (Daejeon, Daegu, Busan, Incheon, Kwangju, or Ulsan) or small city (areas not classified as a big city). Individuals were classified as currently married or never married, with the latter group including those previously married, widowed, or divorced. Shift work was divided into two categories: yes or no (including housewives and students).

We included two daily life restriction variables: sensory system inability and physical inability. Activity restriction 1 indicated difficulties in learning, memory, and concentration; activity restriction 2 indicated difficulties in dressing and taking a bath; activity restriction 3 indicated difficulties in shopping and going to hospital; and activity restriction 4 indicated difficulties in occupation activity. Responses to these questions were categorised as ‘yes’ or ‘no’ in the questionnaire.

Analytical approach and statistics

The Chi-square test and a longitudinal data analysis were used to investigate the impact of the gap between SES and perceived social class on self-rated health and life satisfaction. We ran Generalised estimation equation(GEE). Because our dependent variables were divided in three ordinal categories, Proc Genmod with cumulative logit in the SAS was used. The strength of GEE is that it is a very flexible approach to analyse correlated data from the same subjects over time [19, 20]. It controls for characteristics that change over time, such as confounding variables other than gender. For all analyses, the criterion for significance was a two-tailed p-value ≤ 0.05. All analyses were conducted using the SAS statistical software package version 9.4(SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA).

Self-rated health and life satisfaction (good/normal/bad) were the outcome variables in all GEE models. Covariates of interest from all subjects were added to the model to determine their effects on the probability of reporting self-rated health and life satisfaction. To determine whether the probability of self-rated health and life satisfaction changed over time, we included time (year) in the model as a categorical covariate; the regression coefficient was used to estimate both the change in probability of self-rated health and life satisfaction and independent variables, annually [21].

Results

Table 1 lists the general characteristics of the variables of interest at baseline (Wave 8) for the 11,175 participants included in our analysis. Table 2 shows the general characteristics of the covariates according to the presence of self-rated health and life satisfaction class at baseline (Wave 8). The baseline weighted prevalence of self-rated health and life satisfaction class per year among individuals whose education matched their perceived social class was 12.9 % (high education and high social class), 14.0 % (medium education and medium social class), and 16.0 % (low education and low social class).

Table 1 General characteristics of interesting variables at baseline (Wave 8)
Table 2 General characteristics of interesting variables at baseline (Wave 8) according to self-rated health and life satisfaction

Figures 1 and 2 show the adjusted combined effects of education level and perceived social class on self-rated health and life satisfaction, respectively. Odds ratio (OR) of individuals with low education and low perceived social class was the lowest probability of self-rated health (OR: 0.604; 95 % CI: 0.555–0.656) compared to high education level and high perceived social class. The OR of individuals with low education and middle perceived social class was 0.853 times lower (95 % CI: 0.790–0.922) compared to high education level and high perceived social class. For life satisfaction, OR of individuals with low education and low perceived social class was the lowest probability of self-rated health (OR: 0.068; 95 % CI: 0.063–0.074) compared to high education and high perceived social class, and the OR of individuals with low education and middle perceived social class was 0.071 (95 % CI: 0.066–0.077). The OR of individuals with middle education and middle perceived social class was 0.235 times lower (95 % CI: 0.221–0.251) compared to high education and high perceived social class.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Adjusted combined effects of education and perceived social class on self-rated health. Age, gender, residential region, marital status, shift work, smoking status, alcohol use, sensory system inability, physical inability, activity restriction 1–4, life satisfaction, self-rated health, year were adjusted. Detailed results were included as Additional file 1: Table S1

Fig. 2
figure 2

Adjusted combined effects of education and perceived social class on life satisfaction. Age, gender, residential region, marital status, shift work, smoking status, alcohol use, sensory system inability, physical inability, activity restriction 1–4, life satisfaction, self-rated health, year were adjusted. Detailed results were included as Additional file 1: Table S1

Additionally, we analysed the association of education level and perceived social class on self-rated health and life satisfaction, respectively (Table 3). The adjusted effect of the association of education level and perceived social class on self-rated health and life satisfaction deteriorated across the socioeconomic spectrum.

Table 3 Combined effects of socioeconomic stratum and perceived Social class on Self-Rated Health and Life Satisfaction

Discussion

Summary of main findings

In this study, our primary purpose was to investigate the combined effects of education level and perceived social class on self-rated health and life satisfaction in longitudinal models using a nationally representative sample in South Korea.

There are many research studies on the relationship between traditional socioeconomic status such as income, occupation, and education, and health outcomes. Large socioeconomic differences have been observed in self-rated health. Generally, persons of low socioeconomic status have poorer self-rated health than persons of high socioeconomic status [22, 23].

The meaning of ‘social class’ referring to SES is complex [24]. In general, indicators of SES are meant to provide information about an individual’s access to social and economic resources. Several theories have been put forth to explain observed social gradients in health [25]. Among SES, education is an important determinant of an individual’s work and economic circumstances [26], which are themselves linked to health through specific work conditions and levels of consumption. Education is also associated with health through its connection to health behaviours. The higher one’s level of education, the more likely one is to engage in a range of health-enhancing self-maintenance activities [27]. Years of completed schooling are reported with reasonable ease and reliability and are a meaningful indicator of SES for virtually all adults. Because education is typically completed early in adulthood, it serves as a marker of early life circumstances [27], and no reverse-causation problems result from linking education with health outcomes. Recently, many studies suggest that perceived social class relates to health [28] when explaining health disparities over the past decade.

However, education and perceived social class may not perfectively reflect one’s social position. Thus, a variety of research has been hampered by a lack of social class indicators. According to a study targeting a Total Hip and Knee Replacement (THR/TKR) Dutch population [29], education level (completed level of schooling) had no effect on improvement in quality of life and patient satisfaction, and had a small effect in a similar TKR population. According to a Swedish study, occupational class was also not associated with self-rated health when these other factors were accounted for [30]. Therefore, we included the gap between SES and perceived social class because it is necessary to consider the combined indicators. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to use self-rated health and life satisfaction to examine the combined effect of education and perceived social class of Korean adults, for which we report four key findings.

  1. 1.

    Individuals with a low level of education were significantly more likely to report low self-rated health, compared to individuals with a high level of education.

  2. 2.

    Individuals with a low education level were significantly more likely to report low life satisfaction, compared to individuals with a high education level.

  3. 3.

    Although in the same level of each high, middle, and low education, the possibility of an increase in self-rated health and life satisfaction increased as perceived social class increased from low to high.

  4. 4.

    On the contrary to this, we also saw such a trend that the possibility of an increase in self-rated health and life satisfaction increased as education level increased from low to high, although in the same level of each high, middle and low perceived social class.

Our analysis suggests that it is important to consider the impact of the combined effects on self-rated health and life satisfaction by simultaneously considering education level and perceived social class, as opposed to considering only perceived social class. In addition, our findings suggest that previous studies [31, 32] have overstated the importance of perceived social class in determining health. Our analysis using adjusted combined effects of education level and perceived social class to predict self-rated health and life satisfaction is associated more than using a single simple measure such as perceived social class to predict health outcomes.

Our results have important implications for future research and policy. As life satisfaction and self-rated health capture one aspect of wellbeing and predict future risk of objective health outcomes, further research using different indicators is needed to check the consistency of findings.

In addition, the differing magnitude of inequalities by measure of SES highlights the importance of using multiple measures when quantifying inequalities in self-rated health and life satisfaction. We recommend future research examine the impact of changes to health policy to try to unpack which policies may foster a more equitable distribution of wellbeing. Our findings suggest that mechanisms to buffer the effect of SES may help to reduce socioeconomic inequalities in life satisfaction and self-rated health.

Social relations in the form of social capital, support, and networks have been found to be important determinants of self-rated health [33, 34]. It is assumed that the quality of social interaction results in psychological reactions, which in turn affects health. In addition, life satisfaction is a multidimensional social construct that comprises more than an absence of poor health [35, 36]. The most consistent predictors of life satisfaction are health and wealth status. This finding brings together the evidence that supports the importance of health and wealth on life satisfaction [37, 38].

There are a number of strengths and limitations to this study. The strength of our study is that it is based on a very large sample that included a wide range of variables covering socioeconomic status, such as working conditions and daily life restriction. Thus, the results can be generalised to the South Korean population residing in a city region. In addition, the longitudinal design and sophisticated statistical analyses are a strength of the study. Further, we used a rather novel method to study the association of combined effects for life satisfaction and self-rated health over time.

Nevertheless, this study has some limitations that should be mentioned. First, it was based on self-report measures, which may cause systematic measurement errors (common methods variance). The longitudinal design used in this study diminishes the risk of common method bias. However, this study did not examine causality. For example, the design does not allow for identifying if one’s health effects cause one’s social economic status or vice versa [39, 40]. Second, because personality characteristics are likely to be associated with both gaps and self-rated health and life satisfaction, failure to include them in the statistical models could lead to an exaggeration of the association of interest. Third, although various studies suggest an association between social activity and health outcomes [41, 42] and life satisfaction [43], we could not control for the potential confounding of social relationships and individual partnership due to lack of information in KLIPS data. We were only able to consider physical disabilities and activity restrictions.

Conclusions

This study shows that gap of different socioeconomic spectrum is associated with poor self-rated health and life satisfaction. Our study provides unique concrete evidence that the differing magnitude of education level and perceptions of one’s position in the social hierarchy could have important health implications when quantifying inequalities in life satisfaction and self-rated health. Therefore, policy makers need to consider how to improve the perceptions of one’s position in the social hierarchy as well as education level. In addition, researchers need to develop multi-dimensional measurement tools including education level and subjective social class. Then further research using different indicators could be used to check the consistency of findings.

References

  1. Montano D, Hoven H, Siegrist J. A meta-analysis of health effects of randomized controlled worksite interventions: does social stratification matter? Scand J Work Environ Health. 2014;40:230–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Quon EC, McGrath JJ. Subjective socioeconomic status and adolescent health: a meta-analysis. Health Psychol. 2014;33:433–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Berkman L, Kawachi I. Social epidemiology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Raphael D. Social determinants of health: Canadian perspectives. Toronto: Canadian Scholars’ Press Inc; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  5. House JS. Understanding social factors and inequalities in health: 20th century progress and 21st century prospects. J Health Soc Behav. 2002;43:125–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Dunn JR, Veenstra G, Ross N. Psychosocial and neo-material dimensions of socioeconomic position and health revisited: Predictors of self-rated health in a Canadian national survey. Soc Sci Med. 2006;62:1465–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Singh-Manoux A, Marmot MG, Adler NE. Does subjective social status predict health and change in health status better than objective status? Psychosom Med. 2005;67:855–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Adler NE, Epel ES, Castellazzo G, Ickovics JR. Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: preliminary data in healthy white women. Health Psychol. 2000;19:586–92.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Kim JH, Lee SG, Shin J, Park EC. Impact of the gap between socioeconomic stratum and subjective social class on depressive symptoms: unique insights from a longitudinal analysis. Soc Sci Med. 2014;120:49–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Nordahl H, Osler M, Frederiksen BL, Andersen I, Prescott E, Overvad K, et al. Combined effects of socioeconomic position, smoking, and hypertension on risk of ischemic and hemorrhagic stroke. Stroke. 2014;45:2582–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Quon EC, McGrath JJ. Community, Family, and Subjective Socioeconomic Status: Relative Status and Adolescent Health. Health Psychol. 2014.

  12. Lynch JW, Smith GD, Kaplan GA, House JS. Income inequality and mortality: importance to health of individual income, psychosocial environment, or material conditions. BMJ. 2000;320:1200–4.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Tolson GC BJ, GAY GA, Kowaleski JL. The 1989 revision of the US standard certificates and reports. Natl Cent Health Stat, DHHS Publ No PHS 91-1465 1991, Vital Health Stat:28.

  14. Furnee CA, Groot W, van den Brink HM. The health effects of education: a meta-analysis. Eur J Public Health. 2008;18(4):417–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Zekeri AA. Educational attainment and self-rated health status among single mothers in rural Alabama. Psychol Rep. 2013;113:175–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Kim S-S, Williams DR. Perceived discrimination and self-rated health in South Korea: A nationally representative survey. PLoS One. 2012;7:e30501.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Kim JH, Kim KR, Cho KH, Yoo KB, Kwon JA, Park EC. The association between sleep duration and self-rated health in the Korean general population. J Clin Sleep Med. 2013;9:1057–64.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Lucas RE, Donnellan MB. Estimating the reliability of single-item life satisfaction measures: Results from four national panel studies. Soc Indic Res. 2012;105:323–31.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Liang KY, Zeger SL. Regression analysis for correlated data. Annu Rev Public Health. 1993;14:43–68.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Homish GG, Edwards EP, Eiden RD, Leonard KE. Analyzing family data: A GEE approach for substance use researchers. Addict Behav. 2010;35:558–63.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Arrandale VH, Koehoorn M, MacNab Y, Kennedy SM. Longitudinal analysis of respiratory symptoms in population studies with a focus on dyspnea in marine transportation workers. Int Arch Occup Environ Health. 2009;82:1097–105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Lantz PM, Lynch JW, House JS, Lepkowski JM, Mero RP, Musick MA, et al. Socioeconomic disparities in health change in a longitudinal study of US adults: the role of health-risk behaviors. Soc Sci Med. 2001;53:29–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Yngwe MA, Diderichsen F, Whitehead M, Holland P, Burstrom B. The role of income differences in explaining social inequalities in self rated health in Sweden and Britain. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2001;55:556–61.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Adler NE, Stewart J. Health disparities across the lifespan: meaning, methods, and mechanisms. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2010;1186:5–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Elstad J. Social inequalities in health and their explanations. Norwegian Social Research NOVA Rapport 9/00. Oslo: NOVA; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Lawless M, Buggy CJ, Codd MB. Educational influences on early retirement through disability in Ireland. Occup Med (Lond). 2015;65(4):303–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. In Sociality, Hierarchy, Health: Comparative Biodemography: A Collection of Papers. Edited by Weinstein M, Lane MA. Washington (DC); 2014: The National Academies Collection: Reports funded by National Institutes of Health].

  28. Scott KM, Al-Hamzawi AO, Andrade LH, Borges G, Caldas-de-Almeida JM, Fiestas F, et al. Associations between subjective social status and DSM-IV mental disorders results from the world mental health surveys. Jama Psychiatry. 2014;71:1400–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Keurentjes JC, Blane D, Bartley M, Keurentjes JJB, Fiocco M, Nelissen RG. Socio-economic position Has No effect on improvement in health-related quality of life and patient satisfaction in total Hip and knee replacement: A cohort study. Plos One. 2013;8(3):e56785.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Molarius A, Berglund K, Eriksson C, Lambe M, Nordstrom E, Eriksson HG, et al. Socioeconomic conditions, lifestyle factors, and self-rated health among men and women in Sweden. Eur J Pub Health. 2007;17:125–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Miyakawa M, Magnusson Hanson LL, Theorell T, Westerlund H. Subjective social status: its determinants and association with health in the Swedish working population (the SLOSH study). Eur J Public Health. 2012;22:593–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Kopp M, Skrabski A, Rethelyi J, Kawachi I, Adler NE. Self-rated health, subjective social status, and middle-aged mortality in a changing society. Behav Med. 2004;30:65–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Kawachi I, Kennedy BP, Glass R. Social capital and self-rated health: a contextual analysis. Am J Public Health. 1999;89:1187–93.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Marmot MG, Fuhrer R, Ettner SL, Marks NF, Bumpass LL, Ryff CD. Contribution of psychosocial factors to socioeconomic differences in health. Milbank Q. 1998;76:403–48. 305.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Vanhoutte B. The multidimensional structure of subjective well-being in later life. J Popul Ageing. 2014;7:1–20.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Keyes CL, Shmotkin D, Ryff CD. Optimizing well-being: the empirical encounter of two traditions. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2002;82:1007–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Wiggins RD, Higgs PFD, Hyde M, Blane DB. Quality of life in the third age: key predictors of the CASP-19 measure. Ageing & Society. 2004;24:693–708.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Jivraj S, Nazroo J, Vanhoutte B, Chandola T. Aging and subjective well-being in later life. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2014;69(6):930–41.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Paul KI, Moser K. Unemployment impairs mental health: Meta-analyses. J Vocat Behav. 2009;74:264–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Varekamp I, van Dijk FJ. Workplace problems and solutions for employees with chronic diseases. Occup Med (Lond). 2010;60:287–93.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Glass TA, De Leon CF, Bassuk SS, Berkman LF. Social engagement and depressive symptoms in late life: longitudinal findings. J Aging Health. 2006;18:604–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Isaac V, Stewart R, Artero S, Ancelin ML, Ritchie K. Social activity and improvement in depressive symptoms in older people: a prospective community cohort study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2009;17:688–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Dobrikova P, Pcolkova D, AlTurabi LK, West DJ, Jr. The Effect of Social Support and Meaning of Life on the Quality-of-Life Care for Terminally Ill Patients. Am J Hosp Palliat Care. 2015;32(7):767–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to all the authors of the papers included in this review who responded to our emails to provide additional information about their studies.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tae Hyun Kim.

Additional information

Competing interests

No author has any conflict of interest to declare, financial or otherwise

Authors’ contributions

Acquisition of data, conceived and designed the experiments: JHK. Performed the experiments: JHK, KBY, SGL, ECP. Analysed the data: JHK, ECP, THK. Wrote the manuscript: JHK, KBY, THK. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Jae-Hyun Kim and Ki-Bong Yoo contributed equally to this work.

Additional file

Additional file 1: Table S1.

Adjusted combined effects of Education and Subjective Social Class on Self-Rated Health and Life Satisfaction. (DOC 134 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kim, JH., Yoo, KB., Park, EC. et al. Combined effects of education level and perceived social class on self-rated health and life satisfaction: Results of Korean labor and income panel study wave 8-wave 15. Health Qual Life Outcomes 13, 178 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12955-015-0375-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12955-015-0375-5

Keywords